- Research
- Published:
Residential instability, neighborhood deprivation, and outcomes for children
樱花视频 volume听24, Article听number:听3343 (2024)
Abstract
Background
Residential instability and neighborhood conditions may shape children鈥檚 health and development, but it is unclear whether all residential moves are equally destabilizing, and the extent to which moving to听neighborhoods with different conditions听can improve children鈥檚 outcomes. Most studies estimating causal effects of these factors on children鈥檚 health or development use smaller, geographically constrained, urban cohorts.
Objective
In a racially/ethnically and socioeconomically diverse statewide cohort including urban and rural communities, we investigate effects of residential instability, neighborhood deprivation, and their intersection on childhood educational outcomes.
Methods
We construct a statewide dataset that links North Carolina birth records (2002鈥2005) with lead testing data (2003鈥2015) and 4th grade standardized test scores (2013鈥2016). A composite census tract-level neighborhood deprivation index (NDI) is linked with individuals based on residence at birth, lead testing, and 4th grade. Outcomes of interest are 4th grade test scores in reading and mathematics. We use multinomial propensity scores to estimate effects of residential instability and neighborhood deprivation on test scores.
Results
Children who moved between only high deprivation neighborhoods had lower reading test scores (-0.29 [95% CI: -0.59, -0.015]) compared to children who resided in high deprivation neighborhoods but did not move. Children who resided in a high deprivation neighborhood at birth and subsequently moved to a low deprivation neighborhood(s) had higher test scores compared to those who moved between only high deprivation neighborhoods (1.59 [0.90, 2.28]). Additionally, children who move from high to low deprivation neighborhoods earlier had larger improvements.
Conclusion
Being residentially stable, even while residing in a high deprivation听neighborhood,听is associated with improved educational outcomes. However,听there is also a larger听positive effect of moving from high to low deprivation neighborhoods. Our findings have important implications, particularly given the increasing segregation of neighborhoods by socioeconomic status and the housing affordability crisis in the United States. Partnerships听between housing programs, early childhood听education and services, and health care providers,听which address evictions and broader issues, may help address health inequalities rooted in childhood exposures and experiences.
Background
The Institute of Medicine and National Research Council concluded that 鈥渧irtually every aspect of early human development, from the brain's evolving circuitry to the child's capacity for empathy, is affected by the environments and experiences that are encountered in a cumulative fashion, beginning in the prenatal period and extending throughout the early childhood years鈥漑1]. Focusing on children is essential for understanding cognitive ability, its malleability, and beneficial or adverse effects of different exposures and experiences [2, 3].
A particularly salient experience is where children live and how stable those living environments are. Residential instability contributes to poor health and developmental trajectories [4]. Children who move more frequently are more likely to have poorer physical health, mental health, and behavioral outcomes [5, 6]. Characteristics of neighborhoods 鈥 including poverty, segregation, and crime 鈥 may also contribute to shaping outcomes for children and their families. Sampson et al. (2008) studied effects of concentrated neighborhood disadvantage on the verbal ability of 772 children who participated in the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods [3]. They posit four possible pathways through which neighborhood environments may affect children鈥檚 cognition (and, thereby, test scores): (i) inconsistent maternal parenting practices and/or compromised mental health of caregivers;[8,9,10]; (ii) school quality, which is often linked with neighborhood because public school funding in the US is geographically determined; (iii) restriction of the 鈥渟peech community鈥 to which a child is exposed [11, 12]; and听(iv) limited 鈥渃ommunication infrastructure鈥 and interaction with community members, potentially resulting from distrust, violence, and physical features of the neighborhood [13,14,15]. Other studies have shown that neighborhood context affects verbal ability and that recency of exposure to disadvantaged neighborhoods may affect children鈥檚 test scores (n鈥=鈥600) [7]. However, not all residential moves are necessarily destabilizing for families, as moves that embed children in more highly resourced areas may positively affect their outcomes. Findings from the Moving to Opportunity (MTO) project (n鈥=鈥5,000) suggest that moving to lower poverty neighborhoods earlier in childhood is more likely to benefit children [16, 17].
Situated in the American South, North Carolina (NC) has marked racial/ethnic, socioeconomic, and urban/rural differences in childhood outcomes [18,19,20]. We utilize a large, diverse, population-based sample from NC to investigate the effects of residential instability and neighborhood deprivation on 4th grade test scores. We chose to use 4th grade test scores because they: (i) are early indicators of academic achievement, and therefore useful for facilitating earlier identification of and intervention with 鈥渁t risk鈥 students; (ii) are strong predictors of high school outcomes (e.g., 10th grade test scores, probability of enrolling in advanced courses in high school, and high school graduation); (iii) have, in some cases, been shown to be as predictive of high school outcomes as 8th grade test scores; and (iv) are imperfect but useful proxies for cognitive development [21]. Early educational outcomes are linked with health because overall educational attainment is linked with health (e.g., life expectancy) through various pathways such as income, wealth, insurance status, access to health care, and engaging in health promoting behaviors [22].
Methods
Data
The analytical dataset was constructed from three administrative datasets: (i) the NC detailed birth records (DBR) for 2002鈥2005; (ii) lead surveillance records for years 2003鈥2015; and (iii) end-of-grade (EOG) standardized test scores in reading and mathematics for NC public school students in 4th grade for years 2013鈥2016.
Detailed birth records
We obtained the NC DBR from the NC Department of Vital Statistics/NC Center for Health Statistics (Raleigh, NC). For all recorded live births in NC, the DBR contain data on date and location of birth; maternal health data, demographic data, and residential address at time of birth; and neonate characteristics, including sex, gestational age, birth weight in grams. Studies indicate that DBR听generally provide accurate information for demographic characteristics and birth outcomes [23, 24].
Blood lead surveillance data
Records of blood lead testing were obtained from the Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention Program of the Children鈥檚 Environmental Health Unit in Department of Health and Human Services (Raleigh, NC). Blood lead records contain individual-level data on child characteristics such as age, date of testing, lead concentration in micrograms per deciliter (渭g/dL), and residential address. In the blood lead surveillance data, blood lead levels are recorded as integer values with a limit of detection (LoD) of 1 渭g/dL. Any blood lead levels at or below the LoD听were given a value of 1 渭g/dL. The database does not provide information on why children were screened for lead, however, children should have been screened if they were Medicaid participants or based on their parent鈥檚 responses to the CDC Lead Risk Assessment Questionnaire [25].
End-of-grade (EOG) standardized testing data
Educational records, including EOG test scores, were obtained from the Education Research Data Center housed at Duke University (Durham, NC). In NC, children enrolled in grades 3鈥8 in public schools are administered standardized tests in subject areas of reading and mathematics, typically in May or June of the academic year. These are 鈥渃urriculum-based multiple-choice achievement tests鈥pecifically aligned to the North Carolina Standard Course of Study鈥 [26]. End-of-grade tests consist of multiple choice questions in areas of reading and math. The reading portion of the exam is intended to assess interpretation, critical stance, and connections; the math portion assesses geometry, patterns, relationships, numeration, numerical operations, functions, statistics, and probability (Mathematics) [27]. The educational records also include individual-level data on children, including birth date, in addition to demographic and socioeconomic data, English proficiency, and school and district ID.
Neighborhood deprivation
The neighborhood deprivation index (NDI) was constructed using 2000 and 2010 Census data and 2015 ACS data at the census tract level. The NDI was calculated across NC as previously described, using the first factor loadings from a principal components analysis of multiple variables, including percentages of: households in poverty, female-headed households with dependents, households with annual income鈥<鈥$30,000, households on public assistance, males in management/professional occupation, crowded housing, unemployed, and without a high school education [28]. A census tract was classified as 鈥渉igh NDI鈥 if its NDI was in the top two quintiles of NDI and classified as 鈥渓ow NDI鈥 if its NDI was in the bottom two quintiles of NDI (see Supplemental Material [SM] Table S1). Higher values of NDI indicate greater deprivation.
Study area and sample
In 2010, approximately 8.4% of North Carolina鈥檚 population was Hispanic (of any race); 21.5% was Black or African American; and 68.5% was White [29]. Approximately two-thirds of the population (66.7%) resided in owner-occupied housing, and 66.1% of the population lived in an urban area [29, 30]. The percentage of people in poverty in NC in 2010 was 17.5%, compared to the United States-wide average of 15.3% [31].
There were 88,499 children linked across NC detailed births (2002鈥2005), lead screening (2003鈥2015), and education datasets (2013鈥2016), and geocoded at time of birth, lead testing, and EOG testing. We restricted to children who: were born to self-reported non-Hispanic White (NHW), non-Hispanic Black (NHB), or Hispanic mothers (excluded 3,096 records); did not have limited English proficiency, as it can be complicated to interpret test scores among such children (excluded 5,600 records); had blood lead levels鈥夆墹鈥80 渭g/dL (excluded 8 records); and had blood lead tests between 0 and 6 years of age inclusive (excluded 437 records). We also removed 555 (0.70%) records with missing values for any of the following variables: birthweight percentile for gestational age, preterm birth, Medicaid participation, blood lead test result, child sex, maternal tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal race/ethnicity, maternal age, maternal educational attainment, and maternal marital status at time of child鈥檚 birth.
We also excluded children who did not reside in the same census tract in 3rd grade as in 4th grade (excluded 14,746 records). This was done in an effort to ensure that the neighborhood conditions present in 4th grade were likely also present in 3rd grade. After implementing this restriction, we did not use the 3rd grade census tract information again, resulting in 64,165 individual children retained in the study sample.
Among children who moved between different census tracts, we sought to identify children who had moved between census tracts that were similar with respect to NDI versus children who had moved between census tracts that were dissimilar with respect to NDI. Using census tract of residence at birth, children were linked to NDI calculated using 2000 Census data. Using census tract of residence at time of lead testing, children were linked to NDI calculated using 2000 Census data if lead testing occurred before 2006. If testing occurred in 2006 or later, they were linked to NDI calculated using 2010 Census data. Using census tract of residence at time of 4th grade EOG testing, children were linked to NDI calculated using 2015 American Community Survey (ACS) data.
NDI distributions in 2000, 2010, and 2015 are shown in SM Figure S1. To classify census tracts as low or high NDI, we first calculated quintiles of scaled NDI at time of birth and applied the same quantile range to scaled NDI at time of lead testing and scaled NDI at time of 4th grade standardized testing. A census tract was classified as 鈥渉igh NDI鈥 if the NDI of the tract was in the top two quintiles of scaled NDI, and classified as 鈥渓ow NDI鈥 if the NDI of the tract was in the bottom two quintiles of scaled NDI. A total of 23,823 records were removed for being in the middle quintile at time of birth, or time of EOG testing, or time lead testing (SM Table S1) (remaining n鈥=鈥40,342). NDI at time of birth was used to define ranges delineating high and low NDI because the NDI at time of birth had a wider confidence interval around the mean (compared to NDI at time of EOG testing) and was thus a more conservative approach.
Treatment groups
In the resulting analytical dataset of n鈥=鈥40,342 individuals, we estimate exposure to neighborhood deprivation and residential instability, classifying children as those who (i) resided continuously in the same low deprivation neighborhood; (ii) resided continuously in the same high NDI neighborhood; or (iii) moved between census tracts that were similar vs different with respect to NDI. Thus, children were binned into one of eight categories: high-high-high-stay (N鈥=鈥5,171), high-high-high-move (N鈥=鈥11,537), high-high-low-move (N鈥=鈥2,398), high-low-low-move (n鈥=鈥849), low-low-low-stay (N鈥=鈥2,603), low-low-low-move (N鈥=鈥10,620); 濒辞飞-濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-尘辞惫别 (N鈥=鈥2,956), and 濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-丑颈驳丑-尘辞惫别 (N鈥=鈥1,848) (Table 1). 鈥淗igh/low鈥 represents Census tract NDI at each time point (birth, lead testing, and EOG testing), and 鈥渕ove/stay鈥 indicates whether the child moved Census tracts (SM Table S2). A comparison of characteristics of the original study sample versus the final analytic sample is provided in SM Table S3 and S4.
We excluded individuals who did not fall into one of the eight categories described above (n鈥=鈥2,063), primarily because there were small cell sizes in the remaining categories. We also excluded individuals who did not move census tracts but were classified as having changes in neighborhood deprivation. This could result if an individual did not move but the NDI for their census tract changed over time (or the relative ranking of NDI changed over time), which occurred in a relatively small number of individuals, n鈥=鈥297. The final analytical dataset consisted of 37,982 individuals belonging to one of the eight mutually exclusive categories above.
Statistical analysis
We use propensity scores to estimate the effects of residential instability and neighborhood deprivation on test scores, while accounting for differences between children who experience varying degrees of residential instability and/or exposure to neighborhood deprivation. The propensity score represents the probability of receiving the treatment, conditional on observed characteristics at baseline. Specifically, the distribution of observed baseline covariates should be similar between exposed and unexposed individuals, conditional on the propensity score [32]. To address the potential imbalance in the distribution of observed characteristics between treatment groups, we used a multinomial propensity score model for multiple treatments using generalized boosted models via the mnps function of the twang package in R [33]. We included variables that may be related to the outcome of interest, 4th grade standardized tests scores in reading and mathematics, in propensity score estimation [34, 35]. These include: maternal race/ethnicity (Hispanic, non-Hispanic Black, and non-Hispanic White); birthweight percentile for gestational age; preterm birth, defined as birth prior to 37 weeks gestational age听(yes/no); enrollment in Medicaid at the time of the blood lead test听(yes/no); blood lead level (碌g/dL); sex; maternal smoking during pregnancy听(yes/no); maternal educational attainment (not a high school graduate; high school graduate; college graduate); maternal age at time of birth (15鈥24, 25鈥34, 35鈥44 years); and maternal marital status at the time of birth (married or unmarried) [36, 37].
Treatment groups should, conditional on the propensity score, be balanced with respect to baseline covariates. Imbalance between treatment groups was assessed by comparing the absolute standardized mean differences听(ASMD) between the treatment groups with respect to the observed characteristics, before and after weighting [33]. We also assess the overlap or common support assumption, that is, each individual must have a positive probability of receiving each treatment [38].
To estimate the effect of exposure to residential instability and neighborhood deprivation on test scores, we fit generalized linear models of 4.th grade test scores across the treatment groups that incorporated multinomial propensity score weights using the survey package [39]. One model was fit to compare treatment groups of children that had high NDI at baseline (e.g., high-high-high stay, high-high-high move, high-high-low, and high-low-low). A separate model was fit to compare treatment groups of children that had low NDI at baseline (e.g., low-low-low stay, low-low-low move, 濒辞飞-濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑, and 濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-丑颈驳丑). Regression models controlled for birthweight percentile for gestational age, Medicaid participation, blood lead concentration, child sex, maternal tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal race/ethnicity, maternal age, maternal educational attainment, and maternal marital status at time of child鈥檚 birth. [36, 37].
Reading and math EOG scores were modeled separately. Average treatment effects and their 95% confidence intervals听(CI) are reported.
Sensitivity analysis
We conducted multiple sensitivity analyses. First, to explore potential for selection bias by conditioning selection into the study on having a blood lead test, we removed the criteria of having had a lead test and consider only moves between birth and time of 4th grade testing. Second, we examined associations between NDI, residential stability, and test scores, stratified by urbanicity (urban vs. nonurban neighborhood of residence at baseline [birth]) or race/ethnicity (Hispanic, non-Hispanic Black, and non-Hispanic White) to explore heterogeneity of effects across urbanicity and race/ethnicity. Third, we used proportion of the population in poverty, summarized at the census tract level, instead of NDI to proxy neighborhood SES. Finally, we fit models that adjusted for propensity scores and no other covariates.
This research was governed by a research protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Chicago Illinois and Duke University.
Results
Baseline characteristics of children (i.e., prior to propensity score matching) who resided in high and low NDI census tracts at birth are summarized in SM Tables S5 and S6, respectively. Prior to implementation of propensity scores, compared to children who were residentially stable in high deprivation neighborhoods (high-high-high stay), children who moved between different high deprivation neighborhoods (high-high-high move) had, on average, lower reading scores (SM Table S5). They were also more likely to have lower birthweight percentile for gestational age; be enrolled in Medicaid; have mothers of non-Hispanic Black or Hispanic race/ethnicity; and听have mothers who were younger, reported smoking during pregnancy, were unmarried at time of birth, or did not graduate from high school.
SM Table S6 summarizes observed characteristics of groups of children who resided in low NDI census tracts at baseline. Again, prior to implementation of propensity scores, compared to children who were residentially stable in low deprivation neighborhoods (low-low-low stay), children who moved between different low deprivation neighborhoods (low-low-low move) were more likely to be enrolled in Medicaid; have mothers of non-Hispanic Black or Hispanic race/ethnicity; and have mothers who were younger, were unmarried at time of birth, or did not graduate from high school.
After propensity scores were calculated, imbalance between treatment groups was assessed by calculating the ASMD between the treatment groups with respect to observed characteristics, before and after weighting (SM Tables S7 and S8). After weighting, an ASMD鈥<鈥0.20 suggests that sufficient balance was achieved [33]. After weighting, there were no observed baseline characteristics with an ASMD鈥夆墺鈥0.20 for children who resided in high (SM Table S7) or low NDI (SM Table S8) census tracts at baseline. Boxplots were used to compare the distribution of propensity scores across groups and determine that the overlap assumption was plausibly met [38] (SM Figures S2 and S3).
Residential instability and 4th grade test scores
Average treatment effects of residential instability for children who resided in high and low NDI neighborhoods at baseline are summarized in Table 2 (full regression results are provided in SM Tables S9 and S10). First, we consider children who resided in high deprivation neighborhoods across all three time points. Those who were residentially unstable (high-high-high move) had marginally lower reading scores (鈭0.29 [95% CI: 鈭0.59, 0.015)]; p鈥=鈥0.063) and lower mathematics scores (鈭0.36 [鈭0.65, 鈭0.071]; p鈥=鈥0.015) compared to residentially stable children (high-high-high stay). This suggests that residential instability among children who reside in high deprivation neighborhoods only is associated with lower reading and math test scores.
Next, we consider children who resided in low deprivation neighborhoods across all three time points. Those who were residentially unstable (low-low-low move) had higher reading scores (0.45 [95% CI: 0.056, 0.85)]; p鈥=鈥0.025) and mathematics scores (0.43 [0.027, 0.84]; p鈥=鈥0.037) compared to residentially stable children (low-low-low stay). These findings suggest that residential moves among children who reside in low deprivation neighborhoods only is associated with higher reading and math test scores.
Changing neighborhood environments and 4th grade test scores
Next, we focus on children who were residentially unstable, comparing groups with different levels and timing of exposure to neighborhood deprivation (see Table 3; full regression results are provided in SM Tables S9 and S10). Those children who moved from a high deprivation neighborhood at birth and lead testing to a low deprivation neighborhood at time of EOG testing (high-high-low-move) had higher reading scores (0.93 [0.54, 1.33)]; p鈥<鈥0.001) and mathematics scores (0.78 [0.41, 1.16)]; p鈥<鈥0.001) compared to those who moved between multiple high deprivation neighborhoods (high-high-high move).
Children who moved from a high deprivation neighborhood at birth to low deprivation neighborhoods at time of lead testing and EOG testing (high-low-low-move) had higher reading scores (1.59 [0.90, 2.28)]; p鈥<鈥0.001) and mathematics scores (1.51 [0.84, 2.19)]; p鈥<鈥0.001) compared to those who moved between multiple high deprivation neighborhoods (high-high-high-move).
These comparisons suggest that, among children who reside in high deprivation neighborhoods at time of birth, those who subsequently move to a low deprivation neighborhood have higher test scores compared to their counterparts who move only between neighborhoods that measure high in deprivation. Coefficients also suggest that moving to a lower deprivation neighborhood earlier (e.g., at time of lead testing versus time of end of grade testing) may confer a greater benefit in terms of test scores.
Compared to children who moved between multiple low deprivation neighborhoods (low-low-low move), children who moved from low deprivation neighborhoods at birth and lead testing to a high deprivation neighborhood at time of EOG testing (濒辞飞-濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑) had lower reading scores (鈭0.77 [鈭1.16, 鈭0.38)]; p鈥<鈥0.001) and mathematics scores (鈭0.68 [鈭1.08, 鈭0.28)]; p鈥<鈥0.001) (Table 3).
Finally, children who moved from a low deprivation neighborhood at birth to high deprivation neighborhood(s) at time of lead testing and EOG testing (濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-丑颈驳丑) had marginally lower reading scores (鈭0.62 [鈭1.24, 0.010)]; p鈥=鈥0.054) and lower mathematics scores (鈭0.72 [鈭1.34, 鈭0.11); p鈥=鈥0.022]) compared to children who moved between multiple low deprivation neighborhood (low-low-low move). These comparisons suggest that, among children who reside in low deprivation neighborhoods at time of birth, those who subsequently move to a high deprivation neighborhood have lower test scores compared to their counterparts who move between neighborhoods that are low in deprivation. These results all suggest that moving from a low deprivation neighborhood to a high deprivation neighborhood 鈥 whether earlier or later 鈥 negatively affects reading and mathematics test scores.
Sensitivity analysis
We conducted multiple sensitivity analyses. First, to explore potential for selection bias by conditioning selection into the study on having had a blood lead test, we removed the criteria of having had a lead test. Thus, we only utilize residential information at birth and time of 4th grade testing. Children who moved between high deprivation neighborhoods between birth and 4th grade had lower reading test scores than children who were residentially stable in high deprivation neighborhoods, but the difference was not statistically significant (SM Table S11). Children who moved between low deprivation neighborhoods between birth and 4th grade had higher reading test scores than children who were residentially stable in low deprivation neighborhoods, but again the difference was not statistically significant. Children who moved from a high deprivation neighborhood at birth to a low deprivation neighborhood at time of EOG testing (high-low-move) had higher reading scores (1.25 [0.93, 1.56]; p鈥<鈥0.001) compared to those who moved between high deprivation neighborhoods (high-high-move) (SM Table S12). Children who moved from a low deprivation neighborhood at birth to a high deprivation neighborhood at time of EOG testing (濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-尘辞惫别) had lower reading scores (鈭0.96 [鈭1.25, 鈭0.67]; p鈥<鈥0.001) compared to those who moved between only low deprivation neighborhoods (low-low-move).
For brevity, results of the remaining sensitivity analyses are summarized in Table 4, with detailed results for both reading and math scores provided in the SM. In our second sensitivity analysis, we used proportion of the population in poverty, summarized at the census tract level, instead of NDI to proxy neighborhood SES; results are summarized in Table 4 and SM Tables S13-S14. Third, we examined associations between NDI, residential stability, and test scores, stratified by urbanicity of census tract of residence at time of birth, classified as urban or not urban (e.g., suburban or rural), defined according to Rural鈥揢rban Commuting Area (RUCA) codes [40, 41]. Urbanicity may change over time as the individual moves neighborhoods (or an area becomes more urban). We chose to stratify based on urbanicity at baseline in part because birth was the timepoint with the largest amount of heterogeneity in terms of urbanicity; that is, on average in the analytical dataset, children tended to move into urban rather than rural areas as they aged. Results for urban and nonurban models are provided in Table 4, SM Table 15, and SM Table 16. Fourth, we fit models that were stratified by maternal听race/ethnicity (Hispanic, non-Hispanic Black, and non-Hispanic White) to explore heterogeneity of effects across racial/ethnic groups (Table 4; SM Table S17, and SM Table 18). Finally, we re-fit the main analysis models but adjusted only for propensity scores (i.e., models did not include any additional covariates); results are reported in Table 4, SM Tables S19 and S20.
Discussion
Two comparisons were of particular interest, namely assessing the effects on test scores of residential instability and exposure to neighborhood deprivation. To assess the effect of residential instability, we compared test scores in: (i) children who stayed in the same low deprivation census tract (low-low-low stay) vs those who moved between different low deprivation census tracts (low-low-low move); and (ii) children who stayed in the same high deprivation census tract (high-high-high stay) vs those who moved between different high deprivation census tracts (high-high-high move).
To assess the combined effect of residential instability and exposure to neighborhood deprivation, we compared test scores in children who moved at different time points and to different neighborhood deprivation environments (i.e., high-high-high move vs high-high-low-move; high-high-high-move vs high-low-low-move; low-low-low-move vs 濒辞飞-濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-尘辞惫别; and low-low-low-move vs 濒辞飞鈥揾颈驳丑-丑颈驳丑-尘辞惫别).
Our results with respect to the impact of residential instability and combined residential instability with neighborhood deprivation are visually summarized in Fig. 1. With respect to residential instability, children who moved between high deprivation neighborhoods had lower reading and mathematics test scores compared to children who resided in high deprivation neighborhoods but did not move. This suggests that even if children reside in high deprivation neighborhoods long-term, policies that create greater residential stability may improve educational outcomes.
In contrast, children who moved between low deprivation neighborhoods had higher reading and math scores compared to children who resided in low deprivation neighborhoods but did not move. We are unable to provide a definitive explanation for this phenomenon, but posit that these residential moves are positive moves for the family (e.g., moving to a bigger house or a house with bigger yard).
All of these results, taken together, suggest that the impact of residential instability is highly context dependent. With regard to NDI exposure, among children who moved, those who moved between only low deprivation neighborhoods had higher test scores than children who moved between low and high deprivation neighborhoods. Among children who moved, those who resided in a high deprivation neighborhood at birth and subsequently moved to a low deprivation neighborhood(s) had higher reading and math test scores compared to those who moved between only high deprivation neighborhoods. Thus, exposure to low deprivation neighborhoods is associated with higher test scores, while exposure to high deprivation neighborhoods is associated with lower test scores. Moreover, findings suggest that timing or duration may matter. Namely, children who moved from high to low deprivation neighborhoods earlier in the life course (i.e., by time of lead testing) had larger improvements in test scores compared to those who moved at older ages (i.e., by time of EOG testing).
The estimated effects of residential instability and NDI on test score are consequential. The interquartile range (IQR) in reading and math EOG scores was 13 points for both. Thus, the effects range from 2.2%鈭12.3% of the IQR. For further perspective, maternal education is widely acknowledged to be an important predictor of academic performance [42]. In our models, the estimated effect of mothers completing high school versus not completing high school range from 16.7%鈭20.2% of the IQR. In addition, the effect exposure to poverty, as measured by enrollment in Medicaid, ranges from 11.1%鈭14.0% of the IQR.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first, or one of the first, studies to use administrative datasets to examine residential instability and changing exposure to neighborhood deprivation simultaneously in a population level dataset. There is a large and growing literature on neighborhood socioeconomic conditions and health, developmental, and cognitive outcomes. In a sample of 772 Black/African American children aged between 6 and 12 years old at baseline in Chicago, IL, Sampson et al. (2008) found that prolonged exposure to neighborhood concentrated disadvantage was associated with lower scores on tests of verbal ability. Propensity score-based methods have also been used in observational studies assessing the impact of: kindergarten retention on social-emotional development [43]; school size on mathematics achievement [44]; and high-quality teacher鈥揷hild relationships in kindergarten and math and reading achievement in first grade [45], among others [46].
This study is not without limitations. Compared to our study, many experimental, quasi-experimental, and surveys spanning multiple years have richer covariate data. We did not attempt to control for measures of school characteristics, conditions, or 鈥渜uality鈥. In addition, the children in our dataset are likely biased toward those who are at greatest risk of lead exposure given blood lead surveillance strategies and associated testing patterns employed in the absence of a universal lead screening program in North Carolina. Because the education data are limited to students tested in the North Carolina public school system, we are not able to assess the impact of either residential instability or neighborhood environment on those children enrolled in private school or home-schooled. Additionally, we did not have access to reliable/valid markers of school quality (e.g., student:teacher ratios), or individual- or household-level information regarding home ownership, which would provide important contextual information with respect to residential stability. We did not interpolate NDI values for intercensal years, which may contribute to measurement error (i.e., misclassification). We recognize that standardized test scores are imperfect and incomplete measures of an individual鈥檚 aptitude, comprehension, and overall cognitive abilities; nevertheless, they represent important outcomes. While our findings may be biased due to unmeasured confounders, we attempted to mitigate potential bias by using propensity scores and controlling for maternal- and child-level covariates.
Despite limitations, this study has important strengths. It is one of the first studies of its kind: an observational study that links and leverages existing administrative datasets to better understand how residential context and residential instability over early years in an individual鈥檚 life course relate to standardized test scores. While we may lack richness in covariate data, our approach allows for a large and diverse sample size and includes children from communities across the State of North Carolina, including those in urban, suburban, and rural communities. While not a perfect indicator of cognitive impacts, decrements in EOG scores can have significant consequences. Performance on EOG tests is used to inform decisions about children's educational progress and access to augmented resources. Among children on the low end of the test score distribution, one or two points may mean missing the cut-off for progression to the next grade. For those at the high end of the test score distribution, one or two points may restrict eligibility for advanced and intellectually gifted programs, which rely heavily on test scores to identify students. Additionally, we included multiple sensitivity analyses which led to findings that were generally consistent with the main analysis results.
Conclusion
Our findings have important policy implications. In the US, neighborhoods are increasingly segregated by socioeconomic status, and housing affordability hit an all-time low in 2023 [47, 48]. The housing affordability crisis particularly affects first time homeowners and low-income families, who face challenges finding safe, affordable, stable housing [49]. The housing affordability crisis, combined with high levels of inflation and soaring childcare costs (even outpacing inflation) over the last several years has further eroded residential stability for low-income families with children, placing more children at risk for residential instability and even homelessness [50]. A 2023 report on early childhood housing instability in North Carolina specifically recommended policies that: improve shelters鈥 and housing support programs鈥 abilities to serve families with children and children; increase participation of residentially unstable children in early care and education services; and enhance coordination between housing and early care and education services [51]. The reality that low socioeconomic status students tend to have lower test scores combined with the findings of our analysis 鈥 namely, that residentially unstable students who move among the highest deprivation neighborhoods have the lowest test scores 鈥 underscore the importance of policies to bolster residential stability among lower socioeconomic status families.
Increasingly, researchers, educators, policymakers and health care providers recognize the role of non-clinical factors in shaping health outcomes. Advocating for more equitable housing policy, with an emphasis on both residential stability and the quality of neighborhoods in which children and their families reside, can be an important tool for narrowing health (and educational) inequities and addressing the social correlates of health in concrete ways.
Data availability
Access to the detailed birth records, lead vital statistics data, and educational test score data described in this research are governed by data use agreements and protocols reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Chicago Illinois. The measure of neighborhood deprivation is constructed from publicly available census data are available upon request from the corresponding author. Code to replicate results reported is available upon request from the corresponding author.
Abbreviations
- ASMD:
-
Absolute standardized mean differences
- DBR:
-
Detailed birth records
- EOG:
-
End of grade
- IQR:
-
Interquartile range
- NC:
-
North Carolina
- NDI:
-
Neighborhood deprivation index
References
Shonkoff JP, Phillips D. From neurons to neighborhoods: the science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2000.
Heckman JJ. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. 2006;312:1900鈥2.
Sampson RJ, Sharkey P, Raudenbush SW. Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African-American children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(3):845鈥52.
Kirby JB, Kaneda T. Access to health care: does neighborhood residential instability matter? J Health Soc Behav. 2006;47(2):142鈥55.
Busacker A, Kasehagen L. Association of residential mobility with child health: an analysis of the 2007 National Survey of Children鈥檚 Health. Matern Child Health J. 2012;16(1):78鈥87.
Acosta S. Stable housing is foundational to children's well-Being. In: Center on budget and policy priorities: off the charts. 2022. .
Hicks AL, et al. Sequential neighborhood effects: the effect of long-term exposure to concentrated disadvantage on children鈥檚 reading and math test scores. Demography. 2018;55(1):1鈥31.
Klebanov PK, et al. The contribution of neighborhood and family income to developmental test scores over the first three years of life. Child Dev. 1998;69(5):1420鈥36.
Kling JR, Liebman JB, Katz LF. Experimental analysis of neighborhood effects. Econometrica. 2007;75(1):83鈥119.
Furstenberg FFJ, et al. Managing to make it: urban families and adolescent success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1999.
Ogbu JU. Beyond language: ebonics, proper english, and identity in a Black-American speech community. Am Educ Res J. 1999;36(2):147鈥84.
Griffin P, MS Burns, CE Snow. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. 1998.
Klinenberg E. Heat Wave: A Social Autopsy of Disaster in Chicago. (2nd ed.) University of Chicago Press; 2015.
Ball-Rokeach SJ, Kim Y-C, Matei S. Storytelling neighborhood: paths to belonging in diverse urban environments. Commun Res. 2001;28(4):392鈥428.
Kim Y-C, Ball-Rokeach SJ. Civic engagement from a communication infrastructure perspective. Commun Theory. 2006;16(2):173鈥97.
Sanbonmatsu L, et al. Neighborhoods and academic achievement: results from the moving to opportunity experiment. J Hum Resour. 2006;41(4):649鈥91.
Chetty R, Hendren N, Katz LF. The effects of exposure to better neighborhoods on children: new evidence from the moving to opportunity experiment. Am Econ Rev. 2016;106(4):855鈥902.
Piontak JR, Schulman MD. School context matters: the impacts of concentrated poverty and racial segregation on childhood obesity. J Sch Health. 2016;86(12):864鈥72.
Skinner AC, Slifkin RT. Rural/urban differences in barriers to and burden of care for children with special health care needs. J Rural Health. 2007;23(2):150鈥7.
Nichol G, Hunt H. The persistent and pervasive challenge of child poverty and hunger in North Carolina, N.C.P.R. Fund, Editor. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina; 2021. .
Goldhaber D, Wolff M, Daly T. Assessing the accuracy of elementary school test scores as predictors of students鈥 high school outcomes. CALDER Working Paper 2021. No. 235鈥0821鈥2. .
Case A, AAO Deaton. Life expectancy in adulthood is falling for those without a BA degree, but as educational gaps have widened, racial gaps have narrowed. LID. e2024777118. (1091-6490 (Electronic))..
Buescher PA. Smoking in pregnancy in North Carolina. Maternal characteristics and trends, 1988鈥1994. N C Med J. 1997;58(5):356鈥60.
Buescher PA, et al. The quality of the new birth certificate data: a validation study in North Carolina. Am J Public Health. 1993;83(8):1163鈥5.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Screening young children for lead poisoning: guidance for state and local public health officials. Atlanta: CDC; 1997.
North Carolina Public Schools Accountability Services Division. The North Carolina testing program 2006鈥2007. Raleigh: North Carolina Department of Public Instruction; 2006.
North Carolina Public Schools. Understanding North Carolina end-of-grade testing. In: Assessment brief. Raleigh: North Carolina Division of Public Instruction; 2004.
Messer LC, et al. The development of a standardized neighborhood deprivation index. J Urban Health. 2006;83(6):1041鈥62.
US Census Bureau. 2010 Census: North Carolina profile. 2010. Available from: . Cited 2024 September 25.
US Census Bureau. 2010 Census urban and rural classification and urban area criteria. 2010. Available from: . December 2, 2019. Cited 2021 September 14.
Bishaw A. Poverty: 2010 and 2011. 2012. Available from: . Cited 2024 September 25.
Austin PC. An introduction to propensity score methods for reducing the effects of confounding in observational studies. Multivar Behav Res. 2011;46(3):399鈥424.
McCaffrey DF, et al. A tutorial on propensity score estimation for multiple treatments using generalized boosted models. Stat Med. 2013;32:3388鈥414.
Brookhart MA, et al. Variable selection for propensity score models. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;163(12):1149鈥56.
Brooks JM, Ohsfeldt RL. Squeezing the balloon: propensity scores and unmeasured covariate balance. Health Serv Res. 2013;48(4):1487鈥507.
Bang H, Robins J. Doubly robust estimation in missing data and causal inference models. Biometrics. 2005;61:692鈥972.
Huppler-Hullsiek K, Louis T. Propensity score modeling strategies for the causal analysis of observational data. Biostatistics. 2002;3:179鈥93.
Garrido MM, et al. Methods for constructing and assessing propensity scores. Health Serv Res. 2014;49(5):1701鈥20.
Lumley T. Survey: analysis of complex survey samples. R package version 4.0, 2020.
Hart LG, Larson EH, Lishner DM. Rural definitions for health policy and research. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(7):1149鈥55.
Morrill R, Cromartie J, Hart LG. Metropolitan, urban, and rural commuting areas: toward a better depiction of the U.S. settlement system. Urban Geography. 1999;20(8):727鈥48.
Harding JF, Morris PA, Hughes D. The relationship between maternal education and children鈥檚 academic outcomes: a theoretical framework. J Marriage Fam. 2015;77:60鈥76.
Hong J, Yu B. Effects of kindergarten retention on children鈥檚 social-emotional development: an application of propensity score methods to multivariate, multilevel data. Dev Psychol. 2008;44:407鈥21.
Wyse AE, Keesler V, Schneider B. Assessing the effects of small school size on mathematics achievement: A propensity score-matching approach. Teach Coll Rec. 2008;110:1879鈥900.
McCormick MP, et al. Teacher-child relationships and academic achievement: a multilevel propensity score model approach. J Sch Psychol. 2013;51:611鈥24.
Graham SE, Kurlaender M. Using propensity scores in educational research: general principles and practical applications. J Educ Res. 2011;104(5):340鈥53.
Fry R, P Taylor. The rise of residential segregation by income. 2012. Available from: .听Cited 2019 August 22, 2019.
Ludden J. Most homes for sale in 2023 were not affordable for a typical U.S. household. United States: National Public Radio (NPR); 2023. .
Aurand A, et al. Out of reach: the high cost of housing. In: National low income housing coalition. Washington, DC: National Low Income Housing Coalition; 2021.
Valle-Gutierrez L. Kashen J. As federal funds expire, child care prices are soaring. Commentary: care economy 2023.
Stilwell L, et al. Early childhood housing instability and homelessness in North Carolina: landscape and recommendations to address unmet needs. Durham: Margolis Center for Health Policy at Duke University; 2023.听.
Acknowledgements
We thank Claire Osgood for data management expertise and support.
Data sharing plan
Access to the detailed birth records, lead vital statistics data, and educational test score data described in this research are governed by data use agreements and protocols reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Chicago Illinois. The measure of neighborhood deprivation is constructed from publicly available census data are available upon request from the corresponding author. Code to replicate results reported is available upon request from the corresponding author.
Disclaimer
The findings and conclusions in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent the views of the North Carolina Department of Health and Human Services, Division of Public Health.
clinical trial details
Not applicable.
Other
Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities of the National Institutes of Health under Award Numbers R00MD011304 (PI: Bravo) and the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences under Award Number R01ES028819 (PI: Miranda). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
The findings and conclusions in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent the views of the听North Carolina Department of Health and Human Services, Division of Public Health, the听North Carolina Department of Public Instruction, or the听North Carolina Department of Vital Statistics.
Access to the detailed birth records, lead vital statistics data, and educational test score data described in this research are governed by data use agreements and protocols reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Notre Dame. The measure of neighborhood deprivation is constructed from publicly available census data are available upon request. Code to replicate results reported is available upon request.
Funding
Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities of the National Institutes of Health under Award Numbers R00MD011304 (PI: Bravo) and the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences under Award Number R01ES028819 (PI: Miranda). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization, MAB, MLM; methodology, MAB, DZ; formal analysis, DZ, MAB; writing鈥攐riginal draft preparation, MAB; writing鈥攔eview and editing, MLM, DZ; visualization, MLM, MAB; funding acquisition, MAB, MLM. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article鈥檚 Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article鈥檚 Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit .
About this article
Cite this article
Bravo, M.A., Zephyr, D. & Miranda, M.L. Residential instability, neighborhood deprivation, and outcomes for children. 樱花视频 24, 3343 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-20846-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-20846-6