- Research
- Published:
Towards measuring food insecurity stigma: development and validation of the Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale and the Food Support Experiences Scale
樱花视频 volume听24, Article听number:听3349 (2024)
Abstract
Background
Within high income countries, individuals experiencing food insecurity have become increasingly reliant on food support to satisfy household food needs. However, experiencing food insecurity and accessing food support are highly stigmatised, negatively impacting psychological and emotional wellbeing. Being able to quantify this stigma may contribute towards reducing these impacts. This study aimed to develop and validate two novel scales enabling the quantification of stigma concepts within the food insecurity and food support context: (1) the Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale (FISS), which measures the level of self-stigma (and related constructs) that individuals experiencing food insecurity feel regarding their food insecure status; and (2) the Food Support Experiences Scale (FSES), which measures the psycho-social experiences (including the experience of self-stigma) when individuals access a food support service.
Methods
English speaking participants who identified as experiencing food insecurity completed the new FISS (N鈥=鈥211) and FSES (N鈥=鈥123) measures, alongside other validation measures. Exploratory (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) were carried out for both scales. Regressions using latent variables derived from the CFA were used to test convergent and divergent validity. McDonald鈥檚 Omega was used to assess internal reliability and intra-class correlations between initial and retest FISS and FSES scores of a small number of participants (FISS: N鈥=鈥14; FSES: N鈥=鈥8) were used to assess test-retest reliability.
Results
EFA indicated three-factor structures best fit both scales. CFA revealed a good fit of the model for the FISS (15 items; 3 factors: righteous anger, non-disclosure, and stereotype endorsement). Meanwhile, an acceptable-to-poor fit of the model was revealed for the FSES (23 items; 3 factors: self-approval and disclosure, dietary and interpersonal satisfaction, and perceived effectiveness and impact). Importantly, convergent validity was only found for the non-disclosure subscale of the FISS and the self-approval and disclosure subscale of the FSES.
Conclusions
The FISS and FSES provide valid tools for quantifying aspects of stigma relating to the experience of food insecurity and accessing food support respectively. Development of these two scales may provide an important first step towards measuring stigma. developing interventions which reduce this psychological burden, and working to promote psychological wellbeing within populations experiencing food insecurity.
Background
Recently in the UK, punitive welfare reforms driven by austerity, the COVID-19 pandemic and the subsequent cost of living crisis have made it extremely difficult to afford sufficient food to satisfy household needs, leading to increases in food insecurity [1]. Food insecurity runs on a continuum in which anxiety about food acquisition precedes the experience of not having enough food, with significant mental health impacts across all levels of the continuum [2,3,4]. Almost one in five households in the UK reported experiencing food insecurity in June 2023 [1], and as a result, more people are relying on alternative ways of accessing food, including food support services such as food banks and food pantries.
Previous research within high income countries has consistently reported that people experiencing food insecurity and those subsequently accessing food support are highly stigmatised. For example, Pineau et al. [5] conducted a narrative review which explored the interrelation between social stigma, social exclusion and food insecurity among women residing in high-income countries. While they found that carrying the status of food insecurity was enough to warrant stigmatisation, for those accessing food support the experience of stigma appeared to be even more pronounced. Consistent with this, an international scoping review of 15 years of qualitative research by Middleton et al. [6] found that stigma as a result of food bank access was present across high-income countries including the USA [7], Canada [8,9,10,11], New Zealand [12] and the Netherlands [13]. More recently, in a systematic review conceptualising food banking in the UK [14], 13 of the 24 studies that evidenced psychological implications of food bank use explicitly reported the prevalence of stigma in relation to access. The review also reported a number of other psychological, social and physical health impacts affecting individuals accessing food banks including: experiencing negative emotions such as embarrassment, shame, degradation and powerlessness; an inability to access foods suitable for dietary preferences and needs and to promote health and wellbeing; fear of judgement by staff or the public; and a lack of agency and control. It is likely that these impacts work cumulatively to create a negative and stigmatising experience of accessing food support.
Theoretically, to be stigmatised is to possess an attribute that is 鈥榙eeply discrediting鈥 [15], which differentiates one from others who do not possess the attribute. This attribute is generally thought of as undesirable [15, 16]. For the stigmatised population, the effects may be largely felt through two mechanisms: public stigma and self-stigma [17, 18]. Public stigma occurs when large numbers of people within society endorse negative stereotypes about the stigmatised population, leading to prejudices such as anger and fear of the group, and discrimination including both obvious and nuanced mistreatment including being ignored, avoided, stared at or patronised [17, 19]. Meanwhile self-stigma refers to the internalisation of public stigma (also known as internalised stigma), resulting in decreased self-esteem and self-efficacy [18]. In cases in which a stigmatised condition may be concealable (or is not outwardly obvious to others), there may also exist a certain level of anticipated stigma, defined as the expectation of being stigmatised should their condition be revealed, due to awareness of related negative stereotypes [20]. Within the wider literature, stigma has been evidenced to have wide-reaching impacts on health, well-being, and life chances [21].
Applying this theoretical perspective to the food insecurity context, over the last decade, a shifting of state responsibility for the effects of austerity (including unemployment and reliance on state welfare) towards the individual has initiated neoliberal discourses of laziness and undeservingness [22,23,24]. Such discourses trigger negative attitudes towards individuals who are unable to meet societal demands, such as acquiring food to satisfy social norms. These negative attitudes have become normalised into pejorative stereotypes within society (including laziness, lack of education or concern for health, and an inability to implement skills such as budgeting and cooking) with the aid of unsupportive social policy and media misrepresentation of individuals experiencing food insecurity [6, 25,26,27,28]. The presence of these negative stereotypes represents public stigma regarding food insecurity and food bank access, leading to prejudice and discrimination against individuals experiencing food insecurity, working as a form of social exclusion and exacerbating inequalities within communities [17, 19]. For example, Psarikidou et al. [29] highlight that individuals accessing food support services such as food banks are often viewed as 鈥渞eceivers鈥 of food, as opposed to 鈥減urchasers鈥, indicating a compounding lack of autonomy and agency, and resulting in reduced social status. Middleton et al. [6] reinforce this through conceptualisation of the receipt of food from food banks as a gift, drawing on Mauss鈥 [30] theory which states that all gifts are in some way received with an obligation of reciprocation, and to receive a gift with no means or intention to reciprocate presents a threat to social status. Therefore, individuals receiving 鈥榝ree鈥 food from food banks are liable to a hidden cost 鈥 a reduction in social status from those who do the gifting, and resulting stigmatisation [6, 31].
The internalisation of negative stereotypes (self-stigma) is arguably hard to measure due to social desirability bias and participants鈥 lack of awareness of their own implicit attitudes [32, 33]. Yet, reduced self-esteem (an outcome of self-stigma [18]) has been reported within participants experiencing food insecurity and accessing food support across a range of demographics [34,35,36], indicating the likely prominent role of self-stigma in this context. Additionally, as a result of public stigma, research suggests that individuals experiencing food insecurity may themselves create an 鈥極ther鈥 who embodies the negative stereotypes of laziness, undeservingness and incompetence [6, 26, 37], suggesting internalisation of such negative stereotypes (and thus indicating self-stigma). In some cases, this 鈥極ther鈥 opposes their own 鈥榙eserving鈥 situation, thus creating a hierarchy of social status used to stigmatise [6, 26, 37]. This may act as a protection mechanism against their self-stigma and present an outlet for their frustration at their situation. Meanwhile, in other cases, the creation of an 鈥極ther鈥 built from negative stereotypes can make worthy individuals feel as if they do not qualify for food support despite their food insecure status [6, 38], a situation which has been reported as increasing hopelessness and frustration by Meza, Altman [39].
In terms of anticipated stigma, firstly, individuals experiencing food insecurity may wish to detach from society and reject other forms of help due to the shame of disclosing their struggles accessing enough food [40, 41]. Secondly, reluctance to access, or return to food support services such as food banks for fear of stigmatisation is highly prevalent [25, 38, 42]. Taken together, this may be particularly problematic, as those attending food banks may be already consuming a diet lacking in nutritional quality and quantity [43, 44]. Therefore, non-participation in food support may further diminish the nutritional load of the diet, pushing individuals into micronutrient (and potentially macronutrient) deficiency, which has significant physical health impacts.
Hence, it is clear that stigma within populations experiencing food insecurity must be addressed. Yet, quantitative data surrounding the issue are lacking, and to our knowledge, no scales have been developed and validated which measure experiences of stigma in populations with food insecurity. Kindle et al. [45] developed the Food Pantry Stigma Scale through adaptation of the HIV Stigma Scale [46]. This study was based in America, with the term 鈥榝ood pantries鈥 here referring to the model commonly known as 鈥榝ood banks鈥 in the UK. The scale followed the theoretical perspective that increased stigma is reflected by increases in desired social distance from the relevant group. While avoidance is an important negative response which follows the endorsement of stereotypes and stigmatising attitudes [47], food insecurity is not a condition which is conceived to be contagious or dangerous, therefore, social distance may not be the most salient factor in stigmatisation of this group. Additionally, the Food Pantry Stigma Scale has not yet been validated for use, and since the study was conducted in 2019, societal attitudes towards individuals experiencing food insecurity may have significantly changed and awareness of food insecurity significantly increased. Meanwhile the 24-item Paradox of Self-Stigma scale (PaSS-24) offers a useful tool to measure self-stigma in individuals with mental illness. The PaSS-24 [48] was based upon Corrigan鈥檚 social-cognitive model of internalised stigma (otherwise known as self-stigma), which emphasises the role of paradoxical empowerment [17, 18, 48, 49] and shows good psychometric properties. It measures self-stigma through three constructs which are theoretically linked to self-stigma development and maintenance [18, 48]: stereotype endorsement (cognitive level; the degree to which respondents agree with common stereotypes 鈥 e.g. 鈥楶eople with my condition should be banned from certain jobs鈥), non-disclosure (behavioural level; trying to hide the possession of a stigmatising characteristic from others 鈥 e.g. 鈥楤ecause of people鈥檚 preconceptions, I do not speak to anybody about the problems linked to my condition鈥), and righteous anger (paradoxical element; a legitimate level of anger in response to the stigma surrounding the characteristic 鈥 e.g. 鈥業 am angry about the way my condition is caricatured on television鈥). Corrigan鈥檚 [17, 18, 49] model was developed with individuals experiencing various mental health conditions and upon validation of the PaSS-24, the scale was recommended for validation and use within other stigmatised groups [48]. The PaSS-24 scale may have transferability to the context of food insecurity self-stigma, as the endorsement of negative stereotypes about people experiencing food insecurity [6, 26, 37], non-disclosure of low food security status or need to access food support [26, 50,51,52] and anger and frustration about their food situation and lack of ability to change it [39, 53,54,55] have all been observed within individuals experiencing food insecurity in previous research.
Therefore, the aim of this study was to provide a starting point for quantifying concepts of stigma relating to food insecurity and food support access by developing and validating two novel measures. It was deemed appropriate to start this quantification with measuring self-stigma, due to the obvious presence of public stigma, and the significant potential mental health effects of self-stigma, including loss of self-esteem [18]. The first measure, the Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale (FISS), aimed to measure self-stigma and related constructs in individuals experiencing food insecurity through adaptation of the PaSS-24 to the food insecurity context. The second measure, the Food Support Experience Scale (FSES), aimed to measure the psycho-social experience (including the experience of self-stigma) of accessing food from a food support provider, which may be a food bank, food pantry or other type of food support. This includes items measuring self-stigma as a person who accesses food support, alongside other psychological, social and practical aspects of receiving food from the support provider that may contribute to overall experience. There is important nuance between the two measures, with the FISS measuring only self-stigma in people experiencing food insecurity, who may not necessarily be accessing food support, while the FSES measures self-stigma, alongside other aspects, when accessing food support specifically. Developing two separate scales is important for two reasons. Firstly, not all individuals experiencing food insecurity access food support [56, 57], therefore it is important to be able to measure the stigma that this group feel in relation to their food insecurity status. Secondly, for those who do access food support, it is not clear whether the stigma experienced is due to being food insecure per se, or manifests as a result of accessing food support. Therefore, developing measures of self-stigma and related constructs for food insecurity and the overall experience of accessing food support may help to provide important quantitative evidence for stigmatization of individuals experiencing food insecurity and/or accessing food support. In turn, these measures may extend existing measures of food insecurity and contribute towards the development of interventions which focus on promoting health and wellbeing in this vulnerable population.
Methods
Participants (recruitment and eligibility)
The sample size was based on recommendations for between 5 and 10 participants per scale item, as is considered appropriate for factor analysis in the context of scale development [58]. As individuals experiencing food insecurity are known to be a particularly hard to recruit population, it was considered that five participants per scale item would be sufficient. Therefore, we aimed to recruit 90 participants onto the FISS (18 items), and 115 participants onto the FSES (23 items). Participants were largely recruited via public advertisements on Facebook, however, the advertisement was also shared on Twitter (now known as X). Small amounts of recruitment also took place at local food support services using printed fliers, and via the University of Liverpool School of Psychology Experiment Participation Requirement (EPR) scheme, in which first year students are obliged to earn points by volunteering as participants in research being conducted at the university. The study was advertised as a 鈥楩ood Access and Emotions Study鈥. Individuals over the age of 18 years, living in the UK and fluent in English were eligible to take part. Participants also needed to be experiencing moderate to severe food insecurity, which was ascertained through a screening question at the start of the questionnaire. A secondary screening question was also included after completion of the FISS, which allowed continuation onto the FSES only if participants had accessed food support in the last six months (see Sect.听2.2.1 below for more details on screening questions). Participants were reimbursed for their time through voluntary entry into a prize draw for 拢50 supermarket vouchers and/or course credit (through the University of Liverpool School of Psychology EPR scheme). Recruitment took place between November 2022 and June 2023, with this large window reflective of the difficulty in recruiting the required population.
Measures
Screening questions
Participants were required to complete an initial online screening questionnaire in order to verify their food insecurity status. This screening utilised three questions which have been previously adapted from the United States Department of Agriculture鈥檚 Adult Household Food Security Survey Module (USDA AFSSM) [59] by the Food Foundation, a UK thinktank which conducts regular measuring and reporting of food insecurity in the UK through their Food Insecurity Tracker [1]. The three screening questions measure moderate to severe food insecurity within the context of high-income countries. As the measure is only three items, its implementation helped to minimise participant burden. Participants were asked to report whether, in the last six months, themselves or anyone in their household had: [1] 鈥榟ad smaller meals than usual or skipped meals [2], ever been hungry but not eaten, and [3] not eaten for a whole day, because they 鈥榗ould not afford, or get access to food鈥. This enabled participation by people who were experiencing food insecurity due to a number of causes (e.g. low income, geographical lack of access to supermarkets and/or food support, physical lack of access to supermarkets and/or food support). Answering 鈥榊es鈥 to at least one of these three questions indicated some level of moderate to severe food insecurity, enabling participants to proceed onto answering the FISS and FSES items. Answering 鈥楴o鈥 to all three questions automatically terminated the study, and participants were directed to the Participant Debrief and the option to enter a voluntary prize draw, for the opportunity to win 拢50 supermarket vouchers as a reimbursement for their time.
As the FSES measures psycho-social experiences as a result of accessing food support, a secondary screening question was placed before completion of the FSES. This asked whether participants had, in the last six months, 鈥榬eceived food support from a food support provider such as a food bank, food pantry or community food store, hot meal provider (e.g. a soup kitchen), or other form of food support鈥. Answering 鈥榊es鈥 to this question confirmed food support access, enabling participants to proceed onto answering the FSES. If participants answered 鈥楴o鈥, they were not eligible to complete the FSES items, and so were directed to the next set of measures.
Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale (FISS)
An 18-item, 5-point Likert scale (鈥楽trongly disagree鈥 to 鈥楽trongly agree鈥) measuring aspects of self-stigma (stereotype endorsement, non-disclosure and righteous anger) regarding food insecurity was adapted from the Paradox of Self-Stigma scale (PaSS-24) developed and validated by Golay et al. [48] (see Fig.听1). Selection of the PaSS-24 scale for adaptation to the food insecurity context was based on theoretical fit and also on feedback from stakeholders working directly with individuals experiencing food insecurity, which emphasised that any scale used to measure self-stigma within populations experiencing food insecurity should be indirect and impartial where possible, so as not to further exacerbate self-stigma in participants. The PaSS-24 was felt to be a useful tool in this respect because several items are worded in relation to third parties which allows participants to provide responses which indicate their biases towards other people with their stigmatised condition (e.g. by phrasing items as 鈥楶eople who have difficulty accessing food are less useful to society鈥 as opposed to 鈥業 am less useful to society鈥). This may therefore aid in indirectly measuring the internalisation of negative stereotypes about people experiencing food insecurity [17, 18], without contributing to further self-stigma. This was important as previous self-stigma scales in other contexts have been deemed insensitive, resulting in non-completion by participants and limiting their practical utility [49]. Additionally, the righteous anger element from the PaSS-24 scale was deemed an appropriate fit with food insecurity by stakeholders because it allowed participants to embrace their paradoxical empowerment, potentially helping to moderate and minimise potential negative mental health impacts of completing the survey. The PaSS-24 has strong psychometric properties and is validated to measure self-stigma relating to mental illness, with the authors suggesting its transferability to other stigmatised conditions and contexts [48].
Items within the non-disclosure and righteous anger subscales were easily adapted to suit the food insecurity context by modifying the wording. For example, items in the PaSS-24 referring to a stigmatised 鈥榗ondition鈥 were reworded, defining the 鈥榗ondition鈥 as having 鈥榙ifficulty accessing enough food鈥. Wording was also adapted in certain cases to make the questions easy to comprehend (e.g. 鈥榩eople鈥檚 preconceptions鈥 was replaced with 鈥榩eople鈥檚 opinions鈥). Meanwhile, stereotypes of individuals suffering from a mental health condition, as reflected in the items under the stereotype endorsement factor, are different from stereotypes of individuals experiencing food insecurity. Therefore, items in the PaSS-24 classed under the stereotype endorsement factor were adapted to commonly held stereotypes of individuals experiencing food insecurity, including laziness and aversion to work, poor personal choices, a lack of budgeting and cooking skills, and a societal expectation of gratitude for any and all support, despite its quality [23, 25, 37, 60, 61]. Items in the original PaSS-24 which were unrelated to the food insecurity context (e.g. 鈥楾he restricted rights of people with my condition is scandalous鈥), seemed repetitive (e.g. 鈥楾o avoid disagreeable remarks, I use strategies not to have to talk about my condition鈥), or were overly negative or stigmatising themselves (e.g. 鈥榃hy bother making any effort when I am inferior to others?鈥) were not adapted and were excluded. This included two items from each of the three factors from the original version of the PaSS-24 and so an even split of items across the three factors was maintained for analysis. As stated previously, item adaptation and removal of overly negative items was done in response to feedback from external stakeholders in order to make the scale as relevant as possible, and restrict any further negative feeling as a result of completion. Items were then ordered to follow a repeating cycle of non-disclosure, righteous anger, then stereotype endorsement. Higher scores on the FISS indicate higher levels of self-stigma as a result of being food insecure.
Food Support Experiences Scale (FSES)
This scale is designed to measure psycho-social experiences of food support access, including the experience of self-stigma and other social, psychological and practical aspects. A 23-item, 5-point Likert scale (鈥楽trongly disagree鈥 to 鈥楽trongly agree鈥) was developed from previous qualitative studies with participants experiencing food insecurity and/or those accessing food support services [2,3,4, 23, 25, 26, 28, 37, 38, 50, 60, 62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69]. Domains implicated as playing a role in the process of stigmatisation and contributing to overall negative experience of accessing food support were identified. This qualitative research was largely based in the UK, and is synthesised and summarised in Taylor et al. [14] but also includes evidence from a review of qualitative studies from high income countries [6]. As outlined previously, a second screening question was required which ensured participants only had access to this questionnaire if they had 鈥榬eceived food support from a food support provider such as a food bank, food pantry or community food store, hot meal provider (e.g. a soup kitchen), or other form of food support鈥 within the last six months. This wording was used to enable relevance to a range of food support services, not only charitable services. For example, this may include people鈥檚 experiences of accessing food from social businesses where food is paid for (albeit at a reduced price), which may be perceived as a potentially more socially acceptable way of acquiring food. Participants were also required to indicate their main food provider and respond with their access to this provider only in mind. Higher total scores on the FSES indicate lower levels of self-stigma and a more positive overall experience when accessing food support.
Assessment of convergent and divergent validity
Both the Rosenberg Self-Esteem (RSE) scale and the Depression, Anxiety and Stress (DASS-21) scale were used to assess convergent validity. The RSE scale is a validated 10-item, 4-point Likert scale (鈥楽trongly disagree鈥 to 鈥楽trongly agree鈥) measuring self-worth and self-esteem within participants [70]. A total RSE score was calculated (reliability scores: FISS wt = 0.91; FSES wt = 0.92) with higher scores on the RSE scale indicating higher self-reported self-esteem. Self-esteem is considered to be inversely related to stigma [71, 72], therefore the RSE scale was considered appropriate for assessing convergent validity. Scores on the FISS were expected to be negatively correlated with RSE scale scores and scores on the FSES were expected to be positively correlated with RSE score.
The DASS-21 scale [73] was used to identify the presence of depression, anxiety and stress in participants, using a 4-point Likert scale (鈥楴ever鈥 to 鈥楢lmost always鈥). The 21-item version was selected over the 42-item version in order to minimise participant burden. Individual scores were calculated across each of the three subscales: depression (reliability scores: FISS wt = 0.94; FSES wt = 0.95), anxiety (reliability scores: FISS wt = 0.90; FSES wt = 0.91), and stress (reliability scores: FISS wt = 0.89; FSES wt = 0.90). Higher scores on the corresponding subscale indicate higher levels of depression, anxiety and stress. Stigma relating to food support access has been previously associated with negative mental health outcomes including depression, anxiety and stress. Therefore, higher scores on each DASS-21 subscale were expected to correlate positively to FISS scores and correlate negatively with FSES scores [74, 75].
Meanwhile, the International Physical Activity Questionnaire - Short Form (IPAQ-SF) was used to assess divergent validity. The IPAQ-SF is a 7-item, open ended questionnaire measuring physical activity across 3 levels (vigorous, moderate or walking), conducted in the previous 7 days [76]. Scores are calculated for each of the three levels by multiplying the time spent doing physical activity by the number of days on which the activity was conducted. These scores are then weighted using MET levels (walking鈥=鈥3.3 METs, moderate intensity鈥=鈥4 METs, vigorous intensity鈥=鈥8 METs) and summed to get a final (total) score (reliability scores: FISS wt = 0.74; FSES wt = 0.81). Again, the short-from version was selected in order to minimise participant burden. It was expected that level of physical activity would not correlate with FISS or FSES scores.
Procedure
Ethical approval
was gained for the study from University of Liverpool鈥檚 Research Ethics Committee (reference number 11315). The study protocol and analysis strategy were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework before data collection commenced [77].
Survey items for both the FISS and FSES were drafted and then sent to relevant stakeholders currently working with individuals who are experiencing food insecurity for preliminary checking and feedback. Changes were made to survey items to address this feedback, including adaptation of language to better suit the study population (e.g. changing 鈥榝ood insecurity鈥 to 鈥榙ifficulty accessing enough food鈥 in FISS items) and to promote a more positive framing (for example swapping 鈥楪etting food from my food support provider is an undignified experience鈥 for 鈥楪etting food from my food support provider is a dignified experience鈥 in FSES items), thus explaining why higher scores on the FSES indicate a more positive overall experience (including less self-stigmatisation) of accessing food support. Considering the positionality of the research team as outsiders to the phenomenon in question, this changing of language and framing is highly important and relevant so as not to further marginalise the population [78].
The survey items were then uploaded into a single survey on Qualtrics.com, for participants to complete online. Participants were required to first read the participant information sheet and consent to the study, before generating a unique 7-digit ID code which could be used to link survey data in order for test-retest reliability to be established. Participants were asked to formulate their unique ID code from the first two letters of their first name, followed by the first three letters of the month they were born in, and finally the last two letters of their surname. Participants were then asked to answer the initial 3-item food insecurity screening question to ensure they were experiencing moderate to severe food insecurity, as per the eligibility criteria. If participants identified as experiencing moderate to severe food insecurity, they were then asked to provide some demographic data relating to their gender, age, ethnicity, income, education and employment status, living situation, and access to food support (including type of food support accessed, and frequency and format of access). All demographic questions were closed, multiple choice questions, apart from age, which had a validated response format, meaning that participants were limited to entering just two characters. Where appropriate, participants had the option to respond to the demographic questions using free text if they selected the option 鈥極ther 鈥 please specify鈥. Initially, participants completed the FISS, RSE, DASS-21, and IPAQ-SF measures, followed by the FSES. However, due to the length of the overall survey and the resulting level of non-completion, it became apparent that the FSES should be accessed earlier in the questionnaire, to ensure maximum responses. Subsequently, participants completed the FISS, then the FSES, followed by the measures for convergent and divergent validity. At the point of completion of the FSES, the secondary screening question was implemented, and participants who did not identify as having accessed food support within the last six months were directed to the end of the block, skipping the FSES and moving on to complete the measures for convergent and divergent validity.
Once the full questionnaire was completed, participants were directed to the participant debrief, and given the opportunity to enter the prize draw. In order to ascertain test-retest reliability, a sample of participants completed the survey again after between two and nine weeks. The long period included for test-retest again reflects the difficulty in recruiting the relevant population.
Data analysis
Data were analysed with R version 4.3.1, using the 鈥榣avaan鈥 and 鈥榩sych鈥 packages. The FISS and FSES were analysed separately, as they are separate scales, yet the same analysis strategy was used on both scales.
Data preparation
Prior to analyses, responses on the FISS and FSES were assigned a value of 1 to 5 (1鈥=鈥塻trongly disagree, 2鈥=鈥塻omewhat disagree, 3鈥=鈥塶either agree nor disagree, 4鈥=鈥塻omewhat agree, 5鈥=鈥塻trongly agree). Higher scores on the FISS indicated higher levels of self-stigma, while higher scores on the FSES indicated a more positive overall experience (including less self-stigmatisation) of food support access. Four items on the FSES were reverse scored in order for inter-correlations to remain positive between scale items. No items on the FISS required reverse scoring. Sampling adequacy was checked on both scales using the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) statistic and Bartlett鈥檚 Test for Sphericity was used to assess whether correlations were sufficient to run factor analysis (p鈥<鈥0.01). Each dataset was randomly split into 2 groups for exploratory factor analysis (EFA datasets: FISS n鈥=鈥109; FSES n鈥=鈥62) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA datasets: FISS n鈥=鈥102; FSES n鈥=鈥61).
Exploratory factor analysis
Parallel analysis was used to estimate the number of factors within each dataset. Exploratory factor analyses with an oblique rotation were then conducted on the EFA datasets, as factors were expected to correlate with each other. Items with factor loadings鈥<鈥0.4 were discarded [79], as were those that cross-loaded, namely had two or more factor loadings of >鈥0.32 [80].
Confirmatory factor analysis
Confirmatory factor analyses were subsequently performed using a maximum likelihood with Satorra Bentler correction (MLM) fitting method. The fit of the model was assessed using the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), the Root Mean Square of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Standardised Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). The values for the CFI and TLI were considered good if above 0.95 and acceptable if above 0.90 [81]. The RMSEA indicated a good fit for the model if the score was <鈥0.06 and a moderate but acceptable fit if the score lay between 0.06 and 0.08 [81]. The SRMR indicated a good fit if the score was <鈥0.08 [81]. In the case that the model fit could be improved, modification indices were examined and covariance pathways between residuals were added if items were on the same subscale and the correlated residuals made conceptual sense.
Convergent and divergent validity
Regressions using latent variables derived from the confirmatory factor analysis were used to assess convergent and divergent validity of the FISS and FSES compared with the RSE, DASS-21, and IPAQ-SF.
Internal reliability
McDonald鈥檚 Omega was used to assess internal reliability for both the FISS and FSES, with values acceptable if above 0.7 [82]. Omega Total was calculated for each observed subscale. Omega Hierarchical was also computed for the full scales to explore the extent to which variance attributed to a common factor (鈥檊鈥) was reliable. This allowed us to ascertain whether a total score of the scale can be reliably used, or whether scoring of individual subscales would be more appropriate.
Test-retest reliability
Test-retest reliability was assessed through examination of the intra-class correlation between initial and retest FISS and FSES scores. Intra-class correlations of 0.60 or higher were considered to show good test-retest reliability [83].
Results
The online survey achieved 385 responses, yet not all responses were usable due to failure to complete all the FISS and/or FSES items, resulting in incomplete data. A total of 211 participants completed the FISS and were then eligible to continue on to complete the FSES, having passed the food support access screening question. Of these, 123 participants subsequently completed the FSES scale. Participation exceeded the acceptable level of five participants per scale item for both the FISS (18 items; acceptable n鈥=鈥90) and the FSES (23 items; acceptable n鈥=鈥115) [58]. Table听1 shows the demographic characteristics of participants who completed the FISS and FSES scales.
Examining the demographic data, it was noted that 10 participants reported a household income of 拢52,000 or above, thus raising doubt over the food insecurity status of these participants. These high incomes could not be explained by a higher number of earners living in the house and hence these 10 responses were removed, still maintaining over five participants per scale item in each of the FISS and FSES scales [58]. This sensitivity analysis occurred after the data for the FISS and FSES scales had been split into EFA and CFA datasets. Therefore, the responses of the 10 participants reporting a high income were removed from FISS EFA (n鈥=鈥5) and CFA (n鈥=鈥5) datasets by cross referencing with the whole dataset. The new FISS EFA (n鈥=鈥104) and CFA (n鈥=鈥97) datasets were used in the analysis. Similarly, of the 10 participants reporting high income, three completed the FSES scale. Their responses were also removed from FSES EFA (n鈥=鈥1) and CFA (n鈥=鈥2) datasets by cross referencing with the whole dataset. The new FSES EFA (n鈥=鈥61) and CFA (n鈥=鈥59) datasets were then used for the analysis.
Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale (FISS)
Data preparation
The KMO statistic exceeded the acceptable level of 0.7 (KMO鈥=鈥0.82), and Bartlett鈥檚 Test for Sphericity indicated that correlations between items were large enough for factor analysis (X2(153)鈥=鈥950.19; p鈥&濒迟;鈥0.001).
Exploratory factor analysis
The parallel analysis estimated that a three-factor solution may be most applicable to the data. Applying this, two-, three- and four-factor solutions were tested, with the factor analyses revealing that a clear three-factor solution best suited the data, according to factor loadings. The three-factor solution explained 52% of variance within the 18 items. Factor one explained 21% of variance; Eigenvalue鈥=鈥3.83. Factor two explained 21% of variance, Eigenvalue鈥=鈥3.76. Factor three explained 9% of variance; Eigenvalue鈥=鈥1.71. Table听2 provides the item-factor loadings. Items three (鈥楶eople who have difficulty accessing enough food should take responsibility for their situation鈥), six (鈥楶eople who have difficulty accessing enough food are less useful to society鈥), and 18 (鈥楶eople who have difficulty accessing enough food should be grateful for the help they receive, even if it is poor quality or does not suit their needs鈥) did not load onto any factor sufficiently (see Table听2), and were therefore discarded. All other items loaded onto one factor respectively, without any cross-loading (as defined in Sect.听2.4.2). Factors one and two were made up of six items each, and factor three was made up of three items, following the discarding of the three items. The three factors followed the factor structure suggested by Golay et al. [48] in the PaSS-24 scale as expected. Factor one comprised items concerning righteous anger (for example, 鈥業 am really fed up with people鈥檚 ideas about those who have difficulty accessing enough food鈥). Factor two comprised items referring to non-disclosure of food insecurity status (for example, 鈥業 try to avoid situations where my difficulty in accessing enough food might be revealed鈥). Factor three comprised items relating to stereotype endorsement (for example, 鈥楶eople who have difficulty accessing enough food should try to improve their budgeting skills to improve their situation鈥).
Confirmatory factor analysis
Six items were free to load onto the latent factors of righteous anger and non-disclosure respectively, and three items were free to load onto the latent factor stereotype endorsement. The confirmatory factor analysis suggested a good fit of the model to the data (CFI鈥=鈥0.963; TLI鈥=鈥0.955; RMSEA鈥=鈥0.054; SRMR鈥=鈥0.078), without the need for additional modification indices to be used. Standardised factor loadings were all significant (p鈥<鈥0.001) indicating that all items reflected their underlying latent variable appropriately. The resulting 15-item FISS with scoring instructions is provided in the supplementary material. Figure听2 shows the FISS factor model with standardised factor loadings (single-headed arrows) and covariances (double headed arrows). Unlike in the PaSS-24 scale, where all three factors were positively correlated [48], in the FISS scale, only the factors non-disclosure and righteous anger were positively correlated, while correlations between the other factors were negative.
Internal reliability
Factor analysis demonstrated that the FISS is made up of three subscales (non-disclosure, righteous anger, and stereotype endorsement). The McDonald鈥檚 Omega hierarchical value for the full scale was below the acceptable value of 0.7 (wh = 0.67), suggesting that there is not reliable variance in responses produced by a common factor across all sections. Reliability statistics were produced for each subscale and all achieved acceptable estimates (righteous anger wt = 0.90; non-disclosure wt = 0.93; stereotype endorsement wt = 0.73). Taken together, this suggests that FISS scores are reliable across individual subscales, but a reliable total scale score cannot be produced.
Convergent validity
Regressions using latent variables derived from the confirmatory factor analysis showed that the RSE was only significantly associated (unstandardised coefficient = -0.234; s.e. = 0.084; 95% CI = -0.389, -0.069) with items on the non-disclosure subscale, being in the expected negative direction. Associations between the RSE and the remaining two subscales were non-significant. Using scale means as opposed to latent variables, the overall model was significant, explaining 12% of variance in RSE scores (adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.12, F(3,195)鈥=鈥10.01, p鈥&濒迟;鈥0.001). Again, a significant association was only found with items on the non-disclosure subscale (unstandardised coefficient = -0.325; s.e. = 0.082; 95% CI = -0.487, -0.164; p鈥<鈥0.001), with non-significant associations between RSE scores and stereotype endorsement or righteous anger.
Similarly, regressions using latent variables derived from the confirmatory factor analysis showed that depression (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.322; s.e. = 0.078, 95% CI鈥=鈥0.170, 0.474) anxiety (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.160; s.e. = 0.045, 95% CI鈥=鈥0.07, 0.248) and stress (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.206; s.e. = 0.058, 95% CI鈥=鈥0.093, 0.320) all significantly positively correlated with items on the non-disclosure subscale, but correlations with the other two subscales were non-significant. Using scale means as opposed to latent variables, the overall models were all significant (depression: 11% of variance, adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.105, F(3, 194)鈥=鈥8.67, p鈥<鈥0.001; anxiety: 5% of variance, adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.046, F(3, 194)鈥=鈥4.134, p鈥=鈥0.007; stress: 11% variance, adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.111, F(3, 194)鈥=鈥9.18, p鈥&濒迟;鈥0.001). Regressions using scale means also showed that depression (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.350; s.e. = 0.077; 95% CI鈥=鈥0.198, 0.503; p鈥<鈥0.001) anxiety (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.231; s.e. = 0.070; 95% CI鈥=鈥0.094, 0.368; p鈥=鈥0.001) and stress (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.242; s.e. = 0.064; 95% CI鈥=鈥0.115, 0.368; p鈥<鈥0.001) all significantly positively correlated with items on the non-disclosure subscale, but correlations with the other two subscales were non-significant.
Divergent validity
Regressions using the latent variables indicated that IPAQ-SF total scores were significantly correlated to righteous anger (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥1215.564; s.e. = 530.567, 95% CI鈥=鈥175.672, 2255.456) and non-disclosure (unstandardised coefficient = -1101.833; s.e. = 509.227, 95% CI = -2099.899, -103.766) but not significantly associated to stereotype endorsement. Using scale means as opposed to latent variables, the overall model was significant, explaining 11% of variance in IPAQ scores (adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.11, F(3,109)鈥=鈥5.51, p鈥=鈥0.001). Regressions using scale means indicated that IPAQ-SF total scores were significantly correlated to righteous anger (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥171.08; s.e. = 50.23; 95% CI鈥=鈥71.52, 270.64; p鈥<鈥0.001) and non-disclosure (unstandardised coefficient = -150.95; s.e. = 46.60; 95% CI = -243.316, -58.591; p鈥=鈥0.002) but also significantly associated to stereotype endorsement (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥125.29; s.e. = 54.78; 95% CI鈥=鈥16.711, 233.870; p鈥=鈥0.024). Significant correlations found between scores on the FISS and IPAQ-SF indicate poor divergent validity.
Test-retest reliability
Due to difficulties in recruitment, recruiting onto the retest presented a further challenge. As a result, the time frame for retest was 9 weeks, in order to recruit as many individuals as possible. We obtained 19 retest participants for the FISS, but the data for only 14 of these could be linked using the unique ID codes provided in test one and test two. None of the retest cohort were excluded due to reporting high income. Mean scores for the initial test and retest were calculated and test-retest reliability was found to be good (ICC鈥=鈥0.73).
Food Support Experiences Scale (FSES)
Data preparation
The KMO statistic was acceptable (KMO鈥=鈥0.81) and correlations between items were sufficiently large for carrying out factor analysis (X2(253)鈥=鈥1053.65; p鈥&濒迟;鈥0.001).
Exploratory factor analysis
The new EFA dataset for the FSES (n鈥=鈥61) was constructed for use in conducting an exploratory factor analysis. Parallel analysis using the FSES dataset revealed that a two-factor solution may fit the data best. Two-, three- and four- factor solutions were tested, and items falling into each factor were examined and compared for similarity and relevance to each other. Considering the grouping of items into factors and their conceptual representations, the three-factor solution appeared to be the best fit for the FSES items, explaining 60% of the variance across the 23 items. Factor one was responsible for 24% of the variance; Eigenvalue鈥=鈥5.48, factor two explained 21% of the variance; Eigenvalue鈥=鈥4.83, and factor three explained 15% of the variance; Eigenvalue鈥=鈥3.38. Items 5 (鈥楪etting food from my food support provider makes me feel in control鈥), 11 (鈥楪etting food from my food support provider is a dignified experience鈥) and 14 (鈥業 feel equal to others when I get food from my food support provider鈥) cross loaded over two factors and item 12 (鈥業 feel indebted to my food support provider when I get food from them鈥) did not have a sufficient factor loading for any of the factors, therefore these items were discarded. Table听3 displays the factor loadings for the three-factor structure observed.
Subsequently, factor one was made up of eight items, factor two was made up of seven items, and factor three was made up of four items. Items in factor one represented an individual鈥檚 own feelings about themselves when accessing food support (for example 鈥楪etting food from my food support provider makes me feel good about myself鈥) and about concealing their access to food support to others (for example 鈥業 don鈥檛 mind if my family know I get food from my food support provider鈥). Therefore, this factor was labelled 鈥榮elf-approval and disclosure鈥. Items in factor two concerned the actual experience of accessing food support (for example 鈥楪etting food from my food support provider is generally a good experience鈥), but also included items referring to how well the support provided met client needs and preferences (for example, 鈥楪enerally, I get what I need or want from my food support provider鈥), and the interactions with staff at food support service providers (for example 鈥業 feel taken seriously by the staff at my food support provider鈥). This factor was thus labelled 鈥dietary and interpersonal satisfaction with food support provider鈥. Factor three appeared to be made up of items which described the impact of food support access on the present and future circumstances of individuals (for example 鈥業 have a better quality of life since getting food from my food support provider鈥), and was labelled 鈥perceived effectiveness and impact of food support provider鈥.
Confirmatory factor analysis
To conduct confirmatory factor analysis on the FSES data, the new CFA dataset for the FSES was used (n鈥=鈥59). Initially, confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor structure suggested a poor fit of the model to the data (CFI鈥=鈥0.781; TLI鈥=鈥0.748; RMSEA鈥=鈥0.114; SRMR鈥=鈥0.120). Subsequently, covariance pathways based on the modification indices were added to the model (as described in Sect.听2.4.3. above). As a result, the fit of the model was improved yet the resulting model remained an acceptable-to-poor fit (CFI鈥=鈥0.908; TLI鈥=鈥0.890; RMSEA鈥=鈥0.076; SRMR鈥=鈥0.115). All standardised factor loadings were significant. The resulting 19-item FSES with scoring instructions are provided in the supplementary material. Figure听3 shows the FSES factor model with the standardised factor loadings and covariances. The three factors were all positively correlated, as expected.
Internal reliability
The McDonald鈥檚 Omega hierarchical value for the full scale was below the acceptable value of 0.7 (wh = 0.53), again indicating that variance in responses produced by a common factor across all items was not reliable. The McDonald鈥檚 Omega total value was produced for each subscale and all achieved acceptable estimates (self-approval and disclosure wt = 0.93; dietary and interpersonal satisfaction wt = 0.88; perceived effectiveness and impact wt = 0.84). Therefore, to improve reliability the FSES should be scored as three separate subscales.
Convergent validity
Higher scores on the FSES are indicative of a more positive overall experience (including lower levels of self-stigma) when accessing food support. Therefore, it was predicted that scores on the FSES and the RSE scale would be positively correlated, while scores on the FSES and the DASS-21 scale would be negatively correlated. Regressions using latent variables derived from the confirmatory factor analysis, with the corresponding covariance pathways between residuals applied found a positive significant correlation (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.384; s.e. = 0.085; 95% CI鈥=鈥0.217, 0.552) between the factor labelled self-approval and disclosure of the FSES and the RSE, with no other significant correlations found with the other factors. Using scale means instead of latent variables, the overall model (with covariance pathways applied) was significant, explaining 20% of the variance in RSE scores (adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.20, F(3,119)鈥=鈥11.39, p鈥&濒迟;鈥0.001). Again, a positive significant correlation (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥0.396; s.e. = 0.070; 95% CI鈥=鈥0.258, 0.534; p鈥<鈥0.001) between the factor labelled self-approval and disclosure of the FSES and the RSE, with no other significant correlations found with the other factors.
A significant negative correlation was found between self-approval and disclosure on the FSES and the depression (unstandardised coefficient = -0.356; s.e. = 0.088; 95% CI = -0.528, -0.183), anxiety (unstandardised coefficient = -0.095; s.e. = 0.046; 95% CI = -0.184, -0.005) and stress (unstandardised coefficient = -0.233; s.e. = 0.076; 95% CI = -0.381, -0.085) subscales of the DASS-21 scale, with all other correlations being non-significant. Using scale means as opposed to latent variables, the overall models were all significant (depression: 23% of variance, adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.226, F(3, 119)鈥=鈥12.86, p鈥<鈥0.001; anxiety: 7% of variance, adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.070, F(3, 119)鈥=鈥4.005, p鈥=鈥0.009; stress: 11% variance, adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.105, F(3, 119)鈥=鈥5.772, p鈥=鈥0.001). Regressions using scale means also showed that a significant negative correlation was found between self-approval and disclosure on the FSES and the depression (unstandardised coefficient = -0.358; s.e. = 0.062; 95% CI = -0.482, -0.235 p鈥<鈥0.001), anxiety (unstandardised coefficient = -0.137; s.e. = 0.061; 95% CI = -0.258, -0.016; p鈥=鈥0.026) and stress (unstandardised coefficient = -0.218; s.e. = 0.058; 95% CI = -0.332, -0.104; p鈥<鈥0.001) subscales of the DASS-21 scale, with all other correlations being non-significant.
Divergent validity
Using latent variables, no significant correlations were found between the FSES and the IPAQ-SF, indicating good divergent validity of the FSES. Using scale means, the overall model was significant, explaining 14% of variance in IPAQ-SF scores (adjusted R2鈥=鈥0.14, F [3, 64闭鈥=鈥4.764, p鈥=鈥0.005). Regressions using scale means indicated that IPAQ-SF total scores were significantly correlated to dietary and interpersonal satisfaction scores (unstandardised coefficient鈥=鈥209.13; s.e. = 90.76, 95% CI鈥=鈥27.81, 390.45; p鈥=鈥0.025), but no significant associations were found with the other two subscales.
Test-retest reliability
Within the two- to nine- week window for retest, data for 13 retest participants were obtained for the FSES, but the data for only eight of these could be linked using the unique ID codes provided in test one and test two. None of the retest cohort were excluded due to high income. Mean scores for the initial test and retest were calculated and test-retest reliability was found to be good (ICC鈥=鈥0.83) over the few cases obtained.
Discussion
The present study developed and validated two novel scales which contribute towards understanding the psychological impacts of food insecurity and food support access, focusing on the experience of stigma. The FISS scale aimed to measure concepts of self-stigma when experiencing food insecurity according to Corrigan鈥檚 [17, 18, 49] social-cognitive model of internalised stigma (with emphasis on paradoxical empowerment), including stereotype endorsement, non-disclosure, and righteous anger. Meanwhile the FSES scale measures the psycho-social experience, including the experience of self-stigma, of receiving food from a food support provider specifically. Analysis revealed 3-factor structures for both the FISS and FSES scales. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed a good fit of the three-factor model for the FISS scale comprising non-disclosure, righteous anger, and stereotype endorsement subscales, in line with the PaSS-24 scale [48]. Meanwhile, within the FSES scale, acceptable-to-poor fit of the three-factor model was indicated during confirmatory factor analysis, comprising subscales labelled self-approval and disclosure, dietary and interpersonal satisfaction with food support provider, and perceived effectiveness and impact of food support provider. Good test-retest reliability was confirmed across small samples over a two- to nine-week period for both scales.
The development of the FISS and the FSES presents a starting point for measuring stigma and related experiences in the context of food insecurity and accessing food support. In the future, there is potential for these two scales to be used to evaluate interventions to reduce stigma within populations experiencing food insecurity and in working towards a more positive overall experience when accessing food support. Important applications may include: [1] identifying the negative and self-stigmatising aspects of food insecurity or food support access within particular subgroups of the population in order to provide relevant support; and [2] administering the scales both pre and post an intervention which aims to either improve a food support service or address the barriers and issues experienced by individuals identifying as food insecure. Additionally, the FISS and FSES may build upon the existing USDA food insecurity measures by capturing additional dimensions of food insecurity (namely, food insecurity self-stigma and the psychosocial experience (including experiences of self-stigma) when accessing food support), thus enabling examination of the social acceptability of food acquisition. The FISS and FSES measures may also be used alongside USDA measures of food insecurity to explore the associations between stigma experiences and the level of severity of food insecurity.
Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale听(FISS)
In the development of the FISS scale, items from the PaSS-24 scale [48] were adapted for use within the food insecurity context. The three-factor structure observed in the FISS scale analysis is consistent with the factor structure of the PaSS-24, confirming its applicability to self-stigma relating to food insecurity.
However, in the original PaSS-24 scale all three subscales were shown to be positively correlated with each other. In the present context, while non-disclosure and righteous anger were positively correlated, stereotype endorsement was negatively correlated to both non-disclosure and righteous anger. The negative correlation between stereotype endorsement and righteous anger might suggest that those who experience more feelings of anger regarding their stigmatised position may accept fewer negative stereotypes of individuals experiencing food insecurity. Despite opposing Golay et al.鈥檚 [48] theoretical model, this correlation makes logical sense, as individuals who are angrier about the injustice of their stigmatisation may be more likely to refute negative stereotypes about people in their position. In this respect, unlike in the context of mental illness, righteous anger may protect against stereotype endorsement when considering food insecurity status [18, 48]. As such, interventions which aim to empower individuals experiencing food insecurity to acknowledge? the injustice of their stigmatised status and promote righteous anger, while also addressing public misconceptions and presumptuous stereotypes, may be useful in tackling food insecurity stigma.
The RSE and DASS-21 measures were used to assess convergent validity of the FISS scale. It was expected that scores on the FISS scale would be negatively correlated to self-esteem scores on the RSE [71, 72, 84] and positively correlated to scores on the DASS-21 [74, 75]. However, only the subscale non-disclosure significantly correlated with the RSE and the depression, anxiety and stress subscales of the DASS-21, with the correlations being in the expected direction respectively. This indicates that, for individuals experiencing food insecurity, the fear of their status being revealed might be the most salient factor in the development and manifestation of self-stigma, potentially driving lower self-esteem and worsening mental health outcomes. The development of the FISS scale was underpinned by Corrigan鈥檚 social-cognitive model of internalised stigma (as per the original PaSS-24 scale) which encompasses the role of paradoxical empowerment through righteous anger [17, 18, 48, 49]. While food insecurity and food support access have been linked to anger and frustration [39, 53,54,55], Corrigan鈥檚 model may not be the most appropriate representation of stigma experiences in the context of food insecurity, and this could therefore explain the lack of convergent validity for the righteous anger and stereotype endorsement subscales. Nonetheless, this study presents a starting point for the measurement of stigma relating to food insecurity and food support access, and future research may seek to apply alternative theoretical models.
The IPAQ-SF was used to assess divergent validity of the FISS scale. The IPAQ-SF measures the levels of physical activity achieved in a day, and can relate to leisure, work and transport. While it was expected that no significant correlations would be found, regressions using latent variables showed that IPAQ-SF scores correlated positively with righteous anger and negatively with non-disclosure (meanwhile, regressions with scale means also showed an association between IPAQ-SF scores and stereotype endorsement). Lower levels of physical activity have been linked to lower mental health status [85,86,87] which may lead to wanting to conceal food insecurity status in order to protect from further psychological burden. Meanwhile, those who are more physically active may be in a more positive frame of mind, and may be better able to overcome the 鈥榳hy try鈥 effect of low self-efficacy commonly seen in stigmatised groups [84], to embody righteous anger regarding their situation, potentially explaining the positive correlation between IPAQ-SF scores and righteous anger. Further validation of the FISS scale using other concepts to assess divergent validity are recommended.
Food Support Experiences Scale (FSES)
FSES survey items were developed from the synthesis of qualitative studies summarised within two systematic literature reviews exploring food support access [6, 14]. Overall, the model showed acceptable-to-poor fit, which is likely a result of low power. Repeating the survey over larger samples may increase understanding of the psychometric properties and validity of the FSES. As expected, the three subscales correlated positively, suggesting that the overall experience received at a food support provider might be cumulatively affected by not just how an individual feels about themselves when accessing support, but also how satisfied they are with the food obtained and the interactions with staff, and the level of perceived effectiveness of the support on overall well-being. This supports the idea that access to food support may impact on a broad spectrum of social, physical, and psychological outcomes which influence overall wellbeing [14], and indicates that improving just one of these aspects may aid in creating a more positive and less stigmatising experience for those accessing the support.
Again, the RSE and DASS-21 were used to assess convergent validity. Higher scores on the FSES scale were indicative of a more positive overall experience (including lower levels of self-stigma) when accessing food support, therefore higher FSES scores were expected to be positively associated to RSE score but negatively associated with DASS-21 scores. However, self-approval and disclosure was the only factor which correlated significantly with the RSE and the DASS-21, with associations being in the expected direction. This subscale comprises of items which relate to internalised negative stereotypes of individuals accessing food support and drive to conceal the need to access food support. This association indicates that fear of food insecurity status being revealed might be a salient factor in driving lower self-esteem and poor mental health outcomes not just in people experiencing food insecurity, but also in individuals accessing food support. The lack of significant convergence of RSE and DASS-21 scores with the other two subscales (dietary and interpersonal satisfaction with food support provider and perceived effectiveness and impact of food support provider) may relate to the focus of these subscales being less related to the psychological experience of accessing food support and more related to social, cultural and nutritional experiences. Again, further exploration in larger sample sizes and a reassessment of divergent validity is recommended to ascertain greater validity of the FSES.
Limitations
Several limitations of the study have been identified. Firstly, as noted previously, difficulties in recruitment demanded a wide recruitment window spanning seven months. As a result, recruitment was partially carried out over the Christmas period, commonly considered a time of financial strain for many, especially those experiencing food insecurity. Additionally, due to the difficulties in recruiting participants, only small samples were obtained for the test-retest, over a long time period. It is possible that during this time, food insecurity status may have changed as a result of financial circumstances or otherwise, and further validation of the two scales would benefit from exploring test-retest in larger samples, over a shorter time period [88].
Recent data from the UK report that 57% of individuals experiencing food insecurity are women [89]. Therefore, women were overrepresented in both the FISS (78.2%) and FSES (78.8%) data. While food insecurity is a highly gendered issue, with women reported to take on the daily burden of lack of adequate access to food within the household [3, 50, 68, 90], future research may explore the validation of the FISS and FSES scales within male populations in order to maximise relevance.
Quantifying stigma within populations experiencing food insecurity is a novel concept with a significant paucity in evidence. The development and validation of the FISS and FSES act as a starting point to exploring the concepts of self-stigma in people experiencing food insecurity, and the psychosocial experience (including self-stigma) of accessing food from a food support provider. Further work is now needed to verify the use of the measures in other populations. For example, the current study鈥檚 screening measure for the FSES may have resulted in a skewed focus on food support providers such as food banks, food pantries, community kitchens and hot food providers which are traditionally charitable. Future research could therefore aim to validate the scale in the context of people who are accessing other forms of food support, for example from government food assistance programmes, using measurement invariance analyses. This is important as previous research has indicated the prevalence of (welfare) stigma and negative mental health consequences in this context [91, 92]. Further work may also aim to quantitatively measure other aspects of stigma, including public stigma of people experiencing food insecurity and anticipated stigma of accessing food from a food support provider.
The three-item food insecurity screener used in this study was adapted from the USDA AFSSM [59] by the UK Food Foundation [1] in order to identify individuals experiencing moderate to severe food insecurity. While this was an intentional decision in the current study, use of this measure meant that participants who were experiencing marginal food insecurity or implementing coping strategies to cope with food insecurity were not included. Conducting measurement invariance analyses to explore the relevance of the scales to people experiencing marginal food insecurity is an important future direction, as well as focusing on validating the three-item screener measure. Moreover, we used Corrigan鈥檚 theoretical model and the PaSS-24 scale as a basis for the FISS based on theoretical fit and also guided by stakeholder preferences and ethical priorities. However, further quantification of levels of stereotype endorsement and anger in populations experiencing food insecurity compared to people in food security may shed further light on the transferability of this theoretical approach to the food insecurity context. Additionally, the phrasing of the stereotype endorsement subscale in the FISS referred to third parties (e.g. 鈥楶eople who have difficulty accessing food are less useful to society鈥) which may have created issues with the validity of this subscale; an ideal measure of self-stigma would directly assess how a stigmatised person applies negative stereotypes about the condition to themselves. We chose to phrase the items in this way as we were guided by stakeholder preferences and the importance of minimising distress for participants when completing the scale. As stated, the scales developed in this paper provide a first step towards quantification of stigma, and further work to address potential methodological and ethical trade-off is now required.
Conclusion
This study has developed and validated two novel scales: the Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale and Food Support Experiences Scale. Their development represents an important first step in measuring self-stigma and related concepts in the context of food insecurity and food support access respectively, as well as the FSES measuring the overall psycho-social experience of accessing food support. Further validation using larger sample sizes and in different populations is now needed Ultimately, the wider use of these scales may have important implications in working towards reducing stigma experiences in the context food insecurity and promoting more positive experiences and better psychological wellbeing for individuals accessing food support.
Data availability
The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.The materials generated in the current study are available in the supplementary material and in the Open Science Framework repository ().
Abbreviations
- CFA:
-
Confirmatory factor analysis
- CFI:
-
Comparative Fit Index
- DASS-21:
-
Depression, Anxiety and Stress (21-item) scale
- EFA:
-
Exploratory factor analysis
- EPR:
-
Experiment Participation Requirement (scheme)
- FISS:
-
Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale
- FSES:
-
Food Support Experiences Scale
- ICC:
-
Intra-class correlation
- IPAQ-SF:
-
International Physical Activity Questionnaire - Short Form
- KMO:
-
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (statistic)
- MLM:
-
Maximum likelihood with Satorra Bentler correction
- PaSS-24:
-
Paradox of Self-Stigma scale
- RMSEA:
-
Root Mean Square of Approximation
- RSE:
-
Rosenberg Self-Esteem (scale)
- SRMR:
-
Standardised Root Mean Square Residual
- TLI:
-
Tucker-Lewis Index
- USDA:
-
United States Department of Agriculture
- USDA AFSSM:
-
United States Department of Agriculture Adult Food Security Survey Module
References
The Food Foundation. Food Insecurity Tracker: Round 15 2024 [
Garthwaite KA, Collins PJ, Bambra C. Food for thought: An ethnographic study of negotiating ill health and food insecurity in a UK foodbank. Social Science & Medicine. 2015;132:38鈥44.
Thompson C, Smith D, Cummins S. Understanding the health and wellbeing challenges of the food banking system: a qualitative study of food bank users, providers and referrers in London. Soc Sci Med. 2018;211:95鈥101.
Puddephatt J-A, Keenan GS, Fielden A, Reaves DL, Halford JCG, Hardman CA. Eating to survive鈥: a qualitative analysis of factors influencing food choice and eating behaviour in a food-insecure population. Appetite. 2020;147:104547.
Pineau C, Williams PL, Brady J, Waddington M, Frank L. Exploring experiences of food insecurity, stigma, social exclusion, and shame among women in high-income countries: a narrative review. Volume 8. Canadian Food Studies / La Revue canadienne des 茅tudes sur l鈥檃limentation; 2021. 3.
Middleton G, Mehta K, McNaughton D, Booth S. The experiences and perceptions of food banks amongst users in high-income countries: an international scoping review. Appetite. 2018;120:698鈥708.
De Marco M, Thorburn S, Kue J. In a country as affluent as America, people should be eating: experiences with and perceptions of Food Insecurity among Rural and Urban oregonians. Qual Health Res. 2009;19(7):1010鈥24.
Tarasuk VS, Beaton GH. Household Food Insecurity and Hunger among families using Food banks. Can J Public Health. 1999;90(2):109鈥13.
Nugent MA. Journeys to the food bank: Exploring the experience of food insecurity among postsecondary students University of Lethbridge; 2000.
Kratzmann MLV. More than food: an exploration of the food bank experience in the Halifax regional municipality. Dalhousie University; 2003.
Hicks-Stratton C. The experience of food bank usage among women. a phenomenological study Memorial University of Newfoundland; 2004.
McNeill K. Talking with their mouths half full. Food insecurity in the Hamilton community University of Waikato; 2011.
n Der Horst H, Pascucci S, Bol W. The dark side of food banks? Exploring emotional responses of food bank receivers in the Netherlands. Br Food J. 2014;116(9):1506鈥20.
Taylor N, Boyland E, Hardman CA. Conceptualising food banking in the UK from drivers of use to impacts on health and wellbeing: a systematic review and directed content analysis. Appetite. 2024:107699.
Goffman E. Stigma: notes on the management of Spoiled Identity. London, UK: Penguin Books; 1963.
Jones EE. Social stigma: the psychology of marked relationships. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman; 1984.
Corrigan PW, Kerr A, Knudsen L. The stigma of mental illness: explanatory models and methods for change. Appl Prev Psychol. 2005;11(3):179鈥90.
Corrigan PW, Watson AC. The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2002;9(1):35鈥53.
Boyle MP. Enacted stigma and felt stigma experienced by adults who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2018;73:50鈥61.
Quinn DM, Chaudoir SR. Living with a concealable stigmatized identity: the impact of anticipated stigma, centrality, salience, and cultural stigma on psychological distress and health. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2009;97(4):634鈥51.
Link BG, Phelan JC. Stigma and its public health implications. Lancet. 2006;367(9509):528鈥9.
M枚ller C. Discipline and feed: Food banks, Pastoral Power, and the Medicalisation of Poverty in the UK. Sociol Res Online. 2021;26(4):853鈥70.
Surman E, Kelemen M, Rumens N. Ways to care: forms and possibilities of compassion within UK food banks. Sociol Rev. 2021;69(5):1090鈥106.
Williams A, Cloke P, May J, Goodwin M. Contested space: the contradictory political dynamics of food banking in the UK. Environ Plann A: Econ Space. 2016;48(11):2291鈥316.
Garthwaite K. Stigma, shame and 鈥榩eople like us鈥: an ethnographic study of foodbank use in the UK. J Poverty Social Justice. 2016;24(3):277鈥89.
Lawson L, Kearns A. Foodbanks as paradoxes of policy and society. Voluntary Sect Rev. 2021;12(3):439鈥57.
Moraes C, McEachern MG, Gibbons A, Scullion L. Understanding lived experiences of Food Insecurity through a Paraliminality Lens. Sociology. 2021;55(6):1169鈥90.
Strong S. Towards a geographical account of shame: Foodbanks, austerity, and the spaces of austere affective governmentality. Trans Inst Br Geogr. 2021;46(1):73鈥86.
Psarikidou K, Kaloudis H, Fielden A, Reynolds C. Local food hubs in deprived areas: de-stigmatising food poverty? Local Environ. 2019;24(6):525鈥38.
Mauss M. The gift: the form and reason for exchange in archaic societies. London, UK: Routledge Classics; 1990.
Vlaholias EG, Thompson K, Every D, Dawson D, editors. Reducing food waste through charity: exploring the giving and receiving of redistributed food. Wageningen Academic; 2015.
Cama E, Brener L, Wilson H, Von Hippel C. Internalized Stigma Among People Who Inject Drugs. Substance Use & Misuse. 2016;51(12):1664-8.
Bonetto C, Pace D, Bodini L, Colombi M, Van Bortel T, Lasalvia A. Development and psychometric validation of new questionnaires assessing experienced discrimination and internalised stigma among people with Covid-19. Epidemiol Psychiatric Sci. 2022;31.
Shankar-Krishnan N, Fornieles Deu A, S谩nchez-Carracedo D. Associations between Food Insecurity and Psychological Wellbeing, Body Image, Disordered Eating and Dietary habits: evidence from Spanish adolescents. Child Indic Res. 2021;14(1):163鈥83.
Godrich SL, Loewen OK, Blanchet R, Willows N, Veugelers P. Canadian children from Food Insecure households Experience Low Self-Esteem and Self-Efficacy for healthy lifestyle choices. Nutrients. 2019;11(3):675.
Pak T-Y, Kim G. Food stamps, food insecurity, and health outcomes among elderly americans. Prev Med. 2020;130:105871.
Power M, Small N. Disciplinary and pastoral power, food and poverty in late-modernity. Crit Soc Policy. 2021:0261018321999799.
Pybus K, Power M, Pickett KE. We are constantly overdrawn, despite not spending money on anything other than bills and food鈥: a mixed-methods, participatory study of food and food insecurity in the context of income inequality. J Poverty Social Justice. 2021;29(1):21鈥45.
Meza A, Altman E, Martinez S, Leung CW. It鈥檚 a feeling that one is not worth food: a qualitative study exploring the Psychosocial Experience and Academic Consequences of Food Insecurity among College Students. J Acad Nutr Dietetics. 2019;119(10):1713鈥21. e1.
Wu Z, Schimmele CM. Food Insufficiency and Depression. Sociol Perspect. 2005;48(4):481鈥504.
Power EM, Little MH, Collins PA. Should Canadian health promoters support a food stamp-style program to address food insecurity? Health Promot Int. 2015;30(1):184鈥93.
Cloke P, May J, Williams A. The geographies of food banks in the meantime. Prog Hum Geogr. 2017;41(6):703鈥26.
Forde H, Solomon-Moore E. A qualitative study to understand the potential efficacy of an information-based Sugar reduction intervention among low socioeconomic individuals in the UK. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(3).
Keenan GS, Christiansen P, Hardman CA. Household Food Insecurity, Diet Quality, and obesity: an explanatory model. Obesity. 2021;29(1):143鈥9.
Kindle PA, Foust-Newton M, Reis M, Gell M. Food pantries and Stigma: users鈥 concerns and public support. Contemp Rural Social Work J. 2019;11(1).
Reinius M, Wettergren L, Wiklander M, Svedhem V, Ekstr枚m AM, Eriksson LE. Development of a 12-item short version of the HIV stigma scale. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2017;15(1).
Corrigan P, Markowitz FE, Watson A, Rowan D, Kubiak MA. An Attribution Model of Public Discrimination towards Persons with Mental Illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(2):162.
Golay P, Moga M, Devas C, Staecheli M, Poisat Y, Isra毛l M et al. Measuring the paradox of self-stigma: psychometric properties of a brief scale. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2021;20(1).
Corrigan PW, Michaels PJ, Vega E, Gause M, Watson AC, R眉sch N. Self-stigma of mental illness scale鈥攕hort form: reliability and validity. Psychiatry Res. 2012;199(1):65鈥9.
Power M, Small N, Doherty B, Pickett KE. The incompatibility of System and Lifeworld Understandings of Food Insecurity and the Provision of Food Aid in an English City. Voluntas. 2020;31(5):907鈥22.
May J, Williams A, Cloke P, Cherry L. Still bleeding: the variegated geographies of austerity and food banking in rural England and Wales. J Rural Stud. 2020;79:409鈥24.
Purdam K, Garratt EA, Esmail A. Hungry? Food Insecurity, Social Stigma and Embarrassment in the UK. Sociology-the J Br Sociol Association. 2016;50(6):1072鈥88.
Leung CW, Laraia BA, Feiner C, Solis K, Stewart AL, Adler NE et al. The Psychological Distress of Food Insecurity: A Qualitative Study of the Emotional Experiences of Parents and Their Coping Strategies. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2022;122(10):1903-10.e2.
Leung CW, Stewart AL, Portela-Parra ET, Adler NE, Laraia BA, Epel ES. Understanding the psychological distress of Food Insecurity: a qualitative study of children鈥檚 experiences and related coping strategies. J Acad Nutr Dietetics. 2020;120(3):395鈥403.
Lachance L, Sean Martin M, Kaduri P, Godoy-Paiz P, Ginieniewicz J, Tarasuk V, et al. Food insecurity, diet quality, and mental health in culturally diverse adolescents. Ethn Inequalities Health Social Care. 2014;7(1):14鈥22.
Armstrong B, King L, Clifford R, Jitlal M, Ibrahimi Jarchlo A. Food and you 2: Wave 3 Key findings. Food Standards Agency; 2022.
Loopstra R, Lambie-Mumford H. Food banks: understanding their role in the food insecure population in the UK. Proc Nutr Soc. 2023;82(3):253鈥63.
Comrey AL, Lee HB. A first course in factor analysis. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992.
USDA. U.S. Adult Food Security Survey Module. 2024 [
Douglas F, Sapko J, Kiezebrink K, Kyle J, Resourcefulness. Desperation, shame, Gratitude and Powerlessness: common themes emerging from a study of Food Bank Use in Northeast Scotland. AIMS Public Health. 2015;2(3):297鈥317.
Parr S, Hawkins A, Dayson C. The ethical dilemmas of foodbanking: an analysis of the more Than Food programme. J Poverty Social Justice. 2021;29(3):259鈥77.
Douglas F, Maciver E, Yuill C. A qualitative investigation of lived experiences of long-term health condition management with people who are food insecure. 樱花视频. 2020;20(1).
Lambie-Mumford H. The growth of food banks in Britain and what they mean for social policy. Crit Soc Policy. 2018;39(1):3鈥22.
May J, Williams A, Cloke P, Cherry L. Food banks and the production of scarcity. Trans Inst Br Geogr. 2020;45(1):208鈥22.
McBride J, Smith A. 鈥業 feel like I鈥檓 in poverty. I don鈥檛 do much outside of work other than survive鈥: In-work poverty and multiple employment in the UK. Econ Ind Democr. 2021:0143831X211016054.
Osei-Kwasi HA, Nicolaou M, Powell K, Holdsworth M. I cannot sit here and eat alone when I know a fellow Ghanaian is suffering: perceptions of food insecurity among Ghanaian migrants. Appetite. 2019;140:190鈥6.
Power M, Doherty B, Small N, Teasdale S, Pickett KE. All in it together? Community Food Aid in a multi-ethnic context. J Social Policy. 2017;46(3):447鈥71.
Power M, Small N, Doherty B, Pickett KE. Hidden hunger? Experiences of food insecurity amongst Pakistani and white British women. Br Food J. 2018;120(11):2716鈥32.
Purdam K, Esmail A, Garratt E. Food insecurity amongst older people in the UK. Br Food J. 2019;121(3):658鈥74.
Rosenberg M. Rosenberg self-esteem scale. J Relig Health. 1965.
Corrigan PW, Larson JE, R眉sch N. Self-stigma and the why try effect: impact on life goals and evidence鈥恇ased practices. World Psychiatry. 2009;8(2):75鈥81.
Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Barr L. The self鈥搒tigma of Mental Illness: implications for self鈥揺steem and self鈥揺fficacy. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2006;25(8):875鈥84.
Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. 2nd ed. Sydney (NSW): Psychology Foundation of Australia; 1995.
Berger BE, Ferrans CE, Lashley FR. Measuring stigma in people with HIV: Psychometric assessment of the HIV stigma scale露. Research in Nursing & Health. 2001;24(6):518鈥夆撯29.
Palar K, Frongillo EA, Escobar J, Sheira LA, Wilson TE, Adedimeji A, et al. Food Insecurity, internalized Stigma, and depressive symptoms among women living with HIV in the United States. AIDS Behav. 2018;22(12):3869鈥78.
Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sj枚str枚m M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2003;35(8):. 2003;35(8):1381-95.
Open Science Framework. Open Science Framework (Pre-print) [
Wigginton B, Setchell J. Researching stigma as an outsider: considerations for qualitative outsider research. Qualitative Res Psychol. 2016;13(3):246鈥63.
Stevens JP. Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. 4th ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2002.
Tabachnick B, Fidell L. Using multivariate statistics. 4th ed. Needham Heights: Allyn & Bacon; 2001.
Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equation Modeling: Multidisciplinary J. 1999;6(1):1鈥55.
McNeish D. Thanks coefficient alpha, we鈥檒l take it from here. Psychol Methods. 2018;23(3):412鈥33.
Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):284鈥90.
Corrigan PW, Bink AB, Schmidt A, Jones N, R眉sch N. What is the impact of self-stigma? Loss of self-respect and the why try effect. J Mental Health. 2016;25(1):10鈥5.
Jacob L, Tully MA, Barnett Y, Lopez-Sanchez GF, Butler L, Schuch F, et al. The relationship between physical activity and mental health in a sample of the UK public: a cross-sectional study during the implementation of COVID-19 social distancing measures. Ment Health Phys Act. 2020;19:100345.
Kim YS, Park YS, Allegrante JP, Marks R, Ok H, Ok Cho K, et al. Relationship between physical activity and general mental health. Prev Med. 2012;55(5):458鈥63.
Teychenne M, White RL, Richards J, Schuch FB, Rosenbaum S, Bennie JA. Do we need physical activity guidelines for mental health: what does the evidence tell us? Ment Health Phys Act. 2020;18:100315.
Kennedy I. Sample size determination in test-retest and Cronbach alpha reliability estimates. Br J Contemp Educ. 2022;2(1):17鈥29.
Trussell Trust. Hunger in the UK. 2023.
Harvey K. When I go to bed hungry and sleep, I鈥檓 not hungry: children and parents鈥 experiences of food insecurity. Appetite. 2016;99:235鈥44.
Pak T-Y. Welfare stigma as a risk factor for major depressive disorder: evidence from the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program. J Affect Disord. 2020;260:53鈥60.
Inglis G, Jenkins P, McHardy F, Sosu E, Wilson C. Poverty stigma, mental health, and well-being: a rapid review and synthesis of quantitative and qualitative research. J Community Appl Social Psychol. 2023;33(4):783鈥806.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Robbie Davison for his ongoing support with the research. The corresponding author/lead researcher would also like to thank the ESRC for funding the PhD for which this research was conducted.
Funding
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The corresponding author holds a NWSSDTP ESRC CASE studentship听(Grant number:听ES/P000665/1).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
NT was responsible for carrying out the study, developing the scales, recruitment, data analysis, data interpretation and writing the manuscript. CAH contributed towards conceptualisation of the study, developing the scales, and provided feedback on draft copies of the manuscript. EB also provided feedback on draft copies of the manuscript. PC was a major contributor to the statistical analysis. CAH, EB, PC and AS read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval was gained for the study from University of Liverpool鈥檚 Institute of Population Health Sciences Research Ethics Committee (reference number 11315). Informed consent from all participants was obtained prior to participation in the study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The corresponding author, NT is employed by Can Cook Well-Fed, a social business working against food poverty and insecurity. Can Cook Well-Fed act as an NWSSDTP ESRC CASE organisational partner in the corresponding author鈥檚 PhD studentship. AS is also a trustee of Can Cook. CAH reports research funding from American Beverage Association paid to University of Liverpool, and honoraria from International Sweeteners Association and International Food Information Council all for work outside of the submitted manuscript. PC reports research funding from American Beverage Association paid to University of Liverpool. EB reports no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher鈥檚 note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article鈥檚 Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article鈥檚 Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit .
About this article
Cite this article
Taylor, N., Boyland, E., Christiansen, P. et al. Towards measuring food insecurity stigma: development and validation of the Food Insecurity Self-stigma Scale and the Food Support Experiences Scale. 樱花视频 24, 3349 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-20878-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-20878-y