- Research
- Published:
The impact of eating alone on food intake and everyday eating routines: A cross-sectional study of community-living 70-听to听75-year-olds in Sweden
樱花视频 volume听24, Article听number:听2214 (2024)
Abstract
Background
Eating is fundamental not only to survival and health, but also to how humans organise their social lives. Eating together with others is often seen as the healthy ideal, while eating alone is highlighted as a risk factor for negative health outcomes, especially among older adults. This paper, therefore, investigates the relationship between the frequency and subjective experience of eating alone and food-related outcomes among 70-听to听75-year-olds in Sweden.
Methods
A survey was distributed to a nationally representative random sample of 1500 community-living in Sweden, aged 70鈥75 years. Two different constructs of eating alone (objective and subjective) were measured, along with the following food-related outcomes: a food index, intake of food groups, consumption of ready-made meals, number of main meals per day, and body mass index (BMI).
Results
In total, 695 respondents were included in the final sample. A quarter of the respondents were categorised as eating alone, of which a small proportion reported that doing so bothered them. There were no associations between eating alone and food index scores, BMI, or intake frequencies of fruits and berries, or fish and shellfish. However, people eating alone were less likely to report eating three meals per day (OR: 0.53, CI: 0.37鈥0.76, p鈥=鈥0.006), less likely to report higher intake frequencies of vegetables and snacks, sugary foods, and sweet drinks (adjusted OR: 0.68, CI: 0.48鈥0.95, p鈥=鈥0.023 resp. OR: 0.59, CI: 0.43鈥0.81, p鈥=鈥0.001), and more likely to report higher intake frequencies of ready-made meals (adjusted OR: 3.71, CI: 2.02鈥6.84, p鈥<鈥0.001) compared to those eating together with others. The subjective experience of eating alone did not have an impact on food-related outcomes.
Conclusion
Eating alone or with others played a role in participants鈥 food intake, and seemed to influence aspects of the organisation of everyday eating routines rather than overall dietary healthiness or weight status. Our findings add to the previous body of research on commensality, eating alone, and health among the older population, providing insights into the development of future health policies and research.
Introduction
Eating is fundamental not only to survival and health, but also to how humans organise their social lives. As a consequence, eating in the company of others or in solitude can matter [1]. Some countries actively promote sharing meals as part of their public health advice [2,3,4], and the scientific data provide some support for this. The scientific concept used for the practice of eating together is commensality, a practice that is seen as a healthy ideal [1, 5, 6] with a variety of proposed benefits, both social [7] and nutritional [8, 9]. Studies directed at older people eating alone largely confirm this, demonstrating associations with several negative health- and food-related outcomes, which seem to be particularly prominent among this group [7, 10]. Apart from psychosocial aspects, such as an increased risk of depression [11] and cognitive decline [12], eating alone is also associated with lower food diversity [13, 14], lower intake of fruit and vegetables [15], lower caloric intake [16], meal skipping, and both under- and overweight [15]. Research highlighting the negative impacts of eating alone among older people generally covers a wide age interval, where individuals 60 years and above are included in the same study, thereby encapsulating a large variety of health statuses, abilities, and needs [12,13,14,15,16]. However, the links between eating alone and food-related outcomes may vary within the population group classified as older [10], and the needs of this group may also vary. Nevertheless, studies on eating alone and food-related outcomes that particularly target the earlier stages of retirement and old age are scarce [10].
In this paper, we address a potential issue that has been identified which concerns how eating alone is operationalised, that is when eating alone as an abstract concept is transformed into measurable observations. So far in research on commensality, 鈥榚ating alone鈥 has been treated in a unidimensional and objective way, that is simply assessing whether or not a person eats alone (in solitude) or together with someone [10]. This approach has important empirical and theoretical drawbacks. Empirically, qualitative studies demonstrate that older people鈥檚 experiences of eating alone differ markedly. Some report how it makes them feel lonely, miss a partner, and lose interest in both cooking and eating [17, 18]. This can result in simplifications of everyday cooking and the reorganisation of mealtimes, sometimes described as leading to food of lower perceived quality (e.g., convenience foods) [19, 20]. For others, it is an unproblematic routine and natural part of the day, with ready-made meals and other convenience foods being considered positive [20,21,22]. The theoretical drawbacks are evidenced if we turn to the research on social relations,听where the literature demonstrates the importance of distinguishing between being alone in an objective manner and the subjective feelings of loneliness [23]. These two separate constructs can have different health implications [24, 25], yet the commensality literature currently lacks such a distinction. Levels of loneliness (as a subjective experience) are relatively low in Sweden compared to other European countries [26,27,28]. At the same time, almost one third of Swedish older adults live in single households and it is the second most common living arrangement [29]. As such, this study is located in a national context in which many people live alone (objectively) yet a comparatively small group of people feel lonely. However, according to a Nordic study,听most Swedes over the age of 60 years eat most of their meals with someone [30]. This seemed to be related to other aspects of the organisation of daily routines, such as the duration of meals, and if they are eaten in front of the TV, or sitting down at the kitchen table.
To summarise, we have theoretical reasons to anticipate that people can eat alone quite often without being bothered by it, an anticipation that is deduced in the literature on loneliness and social isolation yet unacknowledged in the commensality literature. The subjective experience of eating alone may, therefore, play a role in understanding the relationship between eating alone and previously identified food-related outcomes, such as the intake of different food groups, ready-made meals, weight status, and everyday eating routines. This is once again, similar to the way the subjective experience of loneliness is of importance for the effects of being alone. Against this backdrop, we aimed to investigate the relationship between eating alone (measured both objectively and subjectively) among 70-听to听75-year old, community-living people in Sweden, and food-related outcomes (food index score, intake of food groups, consumption of ready-made meals, number of main meals per day, and body mass index [BMI]).
Methods
This study was based on a cross-sectional, self-reported survey from a random and nationally representative sample of 70-听to听75-year-olds in Sweden. In total, 1500 people were invited to participate and given the opportunity to respond to the survey either digitally or with pen and paper.
Data collection and Respondents
The survey was distributed nationally by post to randomly selected individuals retrieved from the Swedish state personal address register. An invitation letter with information about the study, a QR-code, and a link to reach the web-based survey was sent out in November 2021. Two reminders (December 2021 and January 2022)听were sent to those who had not replied and not actively declined participation. The second and final reminder included a paper copy of the survey and a stamped self-addressed envelope. Data were collected and managed in REDCap听- a secure, web-based software platform, designed to support data capture for research studies [31]. The COVID-19 pandemic was ongoing during the data collection phase, although,听by that time, restrictions had been lifted and the majority of those in Sweden over 70 years of age had been vaccinated. However, around the time of the second reminder, new restrictions on physical contacts were temporarily in place due to a new disease wave [32].
The age category of 70鈥75 years was chosen for mainly two reasons. First, a majority of older people in Sweden (and Europe) are healthy, active, and independent [33, 34]. In line with this, retirement ages are increasing, and people are working beyond retirement age;听the standard Swedish cut-off of 65 years or older therefore seemed too young for our purposes. Furthermore, this study targets an early phase of retirement, a time of finding or having found new routines after working life. It is, therefore, of interest to investigate whether the possible disadvantages of eating alone are evident at this rather early stage of later life. Even though there are people older than 75听years who are still working, an older target group did not seem relevant for this purpose.
Second, including a restricted age category was decided upon based on power calculations that showed that a larger sample size would have been needed to be able to perform age-specific analyses. A power calculation with 95 per cent confidence interval and unknown proportion of people eating alone in the particular age category resulted in a preferred sample size of at least 385 respondents [35]. Considering the decrease in response rates to national surveys over the past years [36], the survey group invited to participate was more than three times larger than needed (n鈥=鈥1500). Exclusion criteria were individuals diagnosed with or under medical investigation for dementia and those living in a long-term care facility;听these were determined through self-reported screening questions. The survey was only distributed in Swedish and informed consent was needed for responses to be included. This resulted in a final study sample of 695 respondents (Fig.听1).
Survey development
The survey was developed in an iterative process. The majority of questions included were re-used (either literally or with minor modifications) from large national studies [37,38,39]. Senior researchers with experiences of research in older peoples鈥 health and nutrition reviewed the survey at an initial stage and provided feedback on its content and questions. The survey was then further developed by holding cognitive interviews [40] with five individuals belonging to the target population, resulting in changed linguistic formulations and descriptions of questions. The questions regarding eating alone or together, that were created from scratch for this survey, were tested in a bachelor thesis in order to evaluate the data collection process, such as subjects鈥 willingness to participate, data entry, and feedback from respondents (provided as additional free-text responses at the end of the survey). Finally, a pilot study of the survey in its entirety, including people from the target population (n鈥=鈥177), was performed. The pilot did not result in any changes to the survey, but it did provide us with a relevant dataset for testing the statistical models. A version of the survey translated into English can be accessed in Supplementary File A. This is attached for reasons of transparency, so that readers can see the questions asked. However, it has not been tested or validated for use in an English-speaking population.
Data and variables
Eating alone
The frequency of eating alone or together with someone was assessed using the question 鈥淗ow often do you eat together with someone?鈥, for which the responses were 鈥淒aily鈥, 鈥淪everal days per week鈥, 鈥淥ne or two days per week鈥, 鈥淥ne or two days per month鈥, 鈥淟ess often or never鈥. The responses to these questions were then transformed into a binary variable, with those reporting eating together with someone daily categorised as eating together and those reporting eating together with someone less often than that categorised as eating alone. The reason for this was that eating together with someone several days per week or less often means that a substantial proportion of meals are eaten alone.
The subjective experience of eating alone was assessed by asking 鈥淲hen you eat alone, does this ever bother you?鈥, for which the responses were 鈥淎lways鈥, 鈥淥ften鈥, 鈥淩arely鈥, 鈥淣ever鈥, or 鈥淚 never eat alone鈥. Here too, a binary variable was constructed. Those responding 鈥淩arely鈥 or 鈥淣ever鈥 were categorised as not bothered. Those who reported that they never ate alone were also categorised as not bothered, since they could not be bothered by something they never did (i.e., they were not exposed to the phenomenon that could possibly influence the dependent variable). The remaining responses were categorised as bothered. Eating alone was not defined or specified in a particular way, e.g., as being alone in the room, at the table, or being the only one eating. Instead, it was up to the respondents to interpret what eating alone meant to them.
Food-related outcomes
The food-related outcomes consisted of a food index, intake of specific food groups, consumption of ready-made meals, number of main meals per day, and BMI. The food index, developed by the Swedish Food Agency and the Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare, intends to capture food intake relevant from a public health perspective with questions that are easy to answer [41]. The index was constructed using a robust process for the purpose of finding accurate indicators of a diet more or less concordant with the Swedish Food Based Dietary Guidelines (FBDGs), which are consistent with the WHO recommendations [42]. The food index contains questions about the intake frequencies of four food groups (vegetables, fruits and berries, fish and shellfish, and snacks,听sugary foods, and sweet drinks). Every question has four response categories which provide points from 0 to 3 (higher points for higher frequency of consumption, except for snacks,听sugary foods, and sweet drinks which is scored in the opposite direction), adding up to a total of 12 points. For vegetables, fruits and berries, and snacks,听sugary foods,听and sweet drinks the response categories are 鈥淭wo or more times per day鈥, 鈥淥nce per day鈥, 鈥淎 few times per week鈥, or 鈥淥nce per week or less often鈥. The response categories for fish and shellfish are 鈥淭hree or more times per week鈥, 鈥淭wice per week鈥, 鈥淥nce per week鈥, or 鈥淎 few times per month or more rarely鈥. So, for example, if a respondent reported consuming fruits and berries two or more times per day, they would receive 3 points for their response. A higher score indicates a diet better aligned with Swedish FBDGs. Respondents needed to have reported their frequency of consumption for the four food groups included in the food index to be able to calculate a score. The food index (sum score) was categorised into three groups based on the criteria set by the Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare: (1) not eating according to recommendations (score: 0鈥4), (2) somewhere in-between (score: 5鈥8), and; (3) eating approximately according to recommendations (score: 9鈥12). Intake frequencies of the four food groups were also analysed separately, using the response categories stated above, but not the scoring system.
Intakes of ready-made meals and main meals were assessed by asking 鈥淗ow often do you eat the following: Ready-made meals, Breakfast, Lunch, Dinner?鈥, for which the responses were 鈥淒aily鈥, 鈥淪everal days per week鈥, 鈥淥ne or two days per week鈥, 鈥淥ne or two days per month鈥, 鈥淟ess often or never鈥. The responses for ready-made meals were categorised as often (鈥淒aily鈥, 鈥淪everal days per week鈥), sometimes (鈥淥ne or two days per week鈥), and rarely or never (鈥淥ne or two days per month鈥, 鈥淟ess often or never鈥). To generate a total number of main meals per day, respondents needed to have responded to each of the three questions regarding main meals. Eating one main meal per day gave one point, making it possible to have a total of 0鈥3 main meals per day. Eating three main meals per day is the conventional meal pattern in Nordic (and other European) countries [30, 43], which the majority of respondents in this study also did. This variable was therefore categorised as binary, divided into eating three [3], or two or fewer meals per day (鈮も2). We treated these two variables as factors related to the organisation of everyday eating routines, both of which influence food and eating activities [44]. Lastly, BMI was estimated using information on self-reported height in centimetres and self-reported weight in kilograms. Respondents were not given specific instructions on how to measure their height or weight.
Other variables
Year of birth, sex (female or male), living situation (cohabiting or living alone), marital status (not married, married/cohabiting, divorced, or widowed), and country of birth (Sweden, Nordic, European, or other) were included in the survey. Educational level was categorised into primary (<鈥10 years), secondary (10鈥12 years) and post-secondary (鈮モ13 years) education. Respondents were also asked to rate their general health status on a five-point scale, ranging from 鈥淰ery good鈥 to 鈥淰ery poor鈥.
Statistical analysis
For all statistical computations, the software R version 4.3.0 was used [45], with embedded functions as well as the R packages 鈥榯idyverse鈥 [46], 鈥榞gplot鈥 [47], 鈥榡tools鈥 [48], 鈥榗ar鈥 [49], 鈥榦rdinal鈥 [50], and 鈥榬companion鈥 [51]. Descriptive statistics were computed with proportions, means, and standard deviation (if continuous variable). Simple and multiple logistic regressions were used to examine associations between the frequency of eating alone (eating alone vs. eating together) and number of main meals per day (binary variable), and simple and multiple ordinal regressions were used to examine the associations between the frequency of eating alone and food index scores, intake of food groups, consumption of ready-made meals (ordinal variables). The association between the frequency of eating alone and BMI (continuous variable) was assessed with simple and multiple linear regression.
In the multiple regression models, we included the subjective experience of eating alone (bothered vs. not bothered) as an independent variable to explore its independent effect on outcomes. Including this variable also meant that we were able to control that the variation explained by the frequency of eating alone (i.e., objective measure) was independent. Adjustments were initially made for the following covariates: sex, living situation, and educational level. Multicollinearity between independent variables was considered by computing variance inflation factors (VIF). Multicollinearity (VIF鈥>鈥2) was found between the main predictor eating alone or together, and the independent variable living situation. Living situation was, therefore, not included as an independent variable in the adjusted models and the models were only adjusted by sex and educational level. The reason for choosing education as the measure of socioeconomic status is that educational level has become an increasingly important social stratifier in post-industrial European welfare states, and is insensitive to reverse causation (i.e., your completed education at timepoint 1 cannot be affected by your health at timepoint 2) [52]. The significance level for all statistical analyses was set at p-value鈥<鈥0.05. Model outputs were presented with p-values and 95 per cent confidence intervals (CI).
Ethical considerations
Approval for this study was sought from and approved by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (Dnr 2021鈥夆垝鈥01988). Informed consent was obtained from all respondents.
Results
Study population
A description of the study population is presented in Table听1. There was a fairly equal distribution of male and female respondents in the study population, however with more women eating alone. More than two thirds of the study population had secondary or post-secondary education (鈮モ10 years), and over 90 per cent of the respondents were born in Sweden. Three quarters rated their general health status as good or very good, one quarter as moderate, and very few rated their health status as poor or very poor. These characteristics were equally distributed among the two groups (eating alone and eating together). Age was also equally distributed in the total sample, as well as between the two groups, and was further treated as one age category. The majority of the respondents, about three quarters, were cohabiting and one quarter were living alone. This coincided with eating alone or together, meaning that most people who were cohabiting were eating together and most people living alone were eating alone.
Objective and subjective constructs of eating alone
The majority of the respondents reported eating together with someone daily (n鈥=鈥503, 72%) and almost 16 per cent (n鈥=鈥109) of the total sample reported that they never ate alone. Over one quarter of the sample reported eating alone most of the time (n鈥=鈥192, 28%), and four per cent (n鈥=鈥30) of the total sample reported eating together with someone less often than once per month. Of those eating alone, the majority rarely or never felt bothered by it, while a small proportion often or always did. Further details of the combination of the objective and subjective constructs of eating alone are presented in Table听2.
Eating alone and food intake
Table听3 shows odds ratios, 95 per cent confidence intervals, and p-values from adjusted ordinal regression analyses of food index scores, food groups (separated), and intake of ready-made meals. The unadjusted ordinal regression model revealed no association between food index scores and eating alone or together (p鈥=鈥0.288, data not shown). The adjusted model confirmed that there was no statistically significant difference in food index categorisation between those eating alone vs. together (OR: 1.03, CI: 0.72鈥1.47, p鈥=鈥0.870). The distribution of the food index scores for the two groups (eating alone vs. eating together) is presented in Supplementary Fig.听1.
No differences between the intake frequencies of fruits and berries (OR: 0.92, CI: 0.67鈥1.27, p鈥=鈥0.608) or fish and shellfish (OR: 0.79, CI: 0.57鈥1.10, p鈥=鈥0.160) were found between those eating alone and those eating together. However, there was a significant difference showing that respondents eating alone were less likely to report higher intake frequencies of vegetables (OR: 0.68, CI: 0.48鈥0.95, p鈥=鈥0.023) and snacks, sugary foods, and sweet drinks (OR: 0.59, CI: 0.43鈥0.81, p鈥=鈥0.001). Respondents eating alone were more likely to report a more frequent intake of ready-made meals (OR: 3.71, CI: 2.02鈥6.84, p鈥&濒迟;鈥0.001).
Eating alone and the number of main meals per day
Table听3 shows the number and proportion of respondents eating three vs. two or fewer main meals per day in the two groups (eating alone vs. eating together), along with odds ratio, 95 per cent confidence interval, and p-value from the adjusted logistic regression model. The majority of respondents eating alone reported eating two or fewer meals per day (56%). In comparison, a majority of those eating together with someone were eating three meals per day (59%). The unadjusted model showed a statistically significant association between number of main meals per day and eating alone or together (p鈥<鈥0.001, data not shown). The adjusted model confirmed this, showing that respondents eating alone were less likely to report eating three meals per day compared to those eating together with someone (OR: 0.53, CI: 0.37鈥0.76, p鈥=鈥0.006).
Eating alone and BMI
The distribution of BMIs among the two groups (eating alone vs. eating together) is presented in Supplementary Fig.听2. Table听3 shows the mean and standard deviation of BMI, coefficient, and p-value from the adjusted linear regression model. The simple linear regression model showed no significant association between BMI and eating alone or together (p鈥=鈥0.27, data not shown). The adjusted model confirmed that there was no significant difference in BMI among respondents eating alone or together (B: 0.55, CI: -0.19-1.29, p鈥=鈥0.14).
Subjective experience of eating alone and food-related outcomes
There were no associations found between the subjective experience of eating alone (being bothered vs. not being bothered) and any of the food-related outcomes, except for the intake frequency of fish and shellfish. Here, those bothered by eating alone were less likely to report higher intake frequencies of fish and shellfish (OR: 0.36, CI: 0.16鈥0.80, p鈥=鈥0.012, data not shown).
Discussion
This study investigated the subjective and objective constructs of eating alone, and its relationship with food-related outcomes. Our results show that over a quarter of the sample of 70-听to听75-year-olds living in Sweden are eating alone most of the time, and a small group of individuals reported being bothered by it. There were no differences identified in food index scores, intake frequencies of fruits and berries, fish and shellfish, or BMI between those categorised as eating alone or eating together with someone. However, participants eating alone reported a less frequent intake of vegetables and snacks, sugary foods, and sweet drinks, a more frequent intake of ready-made meals, and consuming fewer main meals per day than those eating together with someone. Additionally, the subjective experience of eating alone did not influence food-related outcomes, except for intake frequency of fish and shellfish, which was significantly lower among those being bothered by eating alone. Thus, it appears that, in this sample, eating alone is a better predictor of aspects related to the organisation of everyday eating routines rather than the overall healthiness of diet or the participants鈥 weight status.
Previous research has shown multiple negative food-related effects of eating alone among older adults [13,14,15,16], and there may be several reasons for why our study, in contrast, found no associations between eating alone and most food-related outcomes. Firstly, the study sample consists of independent and rather healthy older individuals in an early phase of retirement. It could be that the negative effects of eating alone become more prevalent and more severe in higher age brackets, since previous studies with wider age intervals (60听years and above) have identified such effects [13,14,15,16]. Secondly, the results may also be explained by cultural factors. Cultural individualism has been identified to modify the relationship between loneliness and health outcomes [53], something that may also be applied to the phenomenon of eating alone. Sweden is a country where the majority culture is considered individualistic [54] and single households are common in all adult age groups [29]. Our data,听and that of others鈥, [30] show that commensality is the most common form of eating in Sweden, and qualitative studies of several age groups indeed suggest that commensality remains idealised, appreciated, and desired [55,56,57,58]. However, as we have shown in a qualitative interview study of Swedes aged听70 years and older, experiences and perceptions of eating alone differ widely [20]. Thus, it could simply be the case that for many people in Sweden, eating in solitude is not such a big deal, even if company would be preferred.
One aspect in which our findings agree with previous studies is the association between eating alone and the pattern of consuming fewer main meals per day or meal skipping [15], less frequent intake of vegetables [13, 15], and with descriptions of more frequent intake of ready-made meals [20]. In our study, those eating alone also reported less frequent intake of snacks, sugary foods, and sweet drinks. Even though these food-related outcomes can be related to health, we cannot draw any clear health-related conclusions from our data since they are only based on frequencies. Nevertheless, we do not reject the possibility that these outcomes could be problematic for older people at risk of malnutrition or other health-related issues where intake frequency is relevant. The motivation for consuming ready-made meals may vary for older adults, with factors such as cooking skills (especially among widowed men) and physical limitations affecting the preparation of food potentially impacting the demand for such meals [19, 22, 59]. The pattern of consuming fewer main meals and more ready-made meals seems therefore, first and foremost, to reflect the daily organisation of eating routines, for example indicating a simplification of everyday eating and less time spent cooking. As such, our results are more in line with sociological studies on how social factors are related to the eating routines of everyday life [44, 60] than studies on food intake and meal frequency and health. One particular hypothesis derived from our findings could be that eating alone influences the organisation of everyday eating, for example through eating fewer main meals and more ready-made meals, and that this triggers health implications later in life. Future research will have to test this hypothesis.
It is well documented that people tend to eat more in the company of others, referred to as the social facilitation of eating [61], while eating alone can have the opposite effect [16]. Hypothetically, weight loss could be an effect of eating alone since it is associated with meal skipping [15] and, in the present study, with eating fewer main meals. However, it is currently unclear whether eating alone and weight status are related. Previous research from Japan indicates an increased risk of self-perceived weight loss [62], and an increased risk of both under- and overweight among men eating alone, although not women [15]. As such, our findings add to the uncertainty of these mixed results, with no associations identified between eating alone and BMI.
The subjective experience of eating alone (bothered vs. not bothered) did not have an independent effect on food-related outcomes. This could be explained by the fact that few (only four per cent) in our sample report being bothered by eating alone. Since power calculations were based on the objective measure of eating alone, a larger sample size might have been able to detect a significant effect of the subjective experience of eating alone on food-related outcomes. Consideration of the subjective experience of eating alone in future research is likely to be important for capturing the full extent of links between eating alone and food-related outcomes.
Strengths and limitations
This study is, to our knowledge, the first to consider both the objective and subjective constructs of eating alone. This was conducted through a rigorous and systematic procedure of questionnaire development, where questions were carefully chosen from, or based on, established surveys. To increase validity and reliability,听the survey was further evaluated using pre-testing of eating-alone questions, a pilot study of its entirety, and cognitive interviews. Data collection was meticulously planned and structured to enable a high response rate, and we consider a response rate of almost half of the sample to be decent, albeit lower than optimal. However, a low response rate among lower educational groups, those with lower self-perceived health status, and a very small representation of non-European immigrants complicates the representativeness and thus generalisation of our findings. There is also the possibility that our sample is biased by the overrepresentation of individuals particularly interested in food and meals, since information about the topic of the study was given prior to participation.
The survey was distributed during the COVID-19 pandemic, which may have further impacted the generalisation of our results. Additionally, restrictions on physical contacts changed during the later data collection phase, meaning that the frequency of eating together or alone might have temporarily changed for some participants. Since people aged 70 years or older had been recommended to stay at home and minimise physical contacts with others during the pandemic, patterns of meal company could have been different compared to normal (non-pandemic) circumstances. This situation could also have affected food choice and food intake, for example due to changes in food shopping and access to food. As demonstrated by a recently published scoping review, such effects on older adults in different countries differ markedly [63]. For example, whereas an increased intake of several food groups (e.g. vegetables, sugary foods and snacks) during the pandemic was reported in some studies, others identified higher levels of food insecurity and hence a reduction in food intake.
In this study, BMI is used as an indicator of weight status. BMI has been criticised for not being a fully appropriate metric for evaluating the weight status of a given older patient, yet it has good predictive value at a population level [64] as long as the same cut-offs as for younger adults are not considered appropriate to apply听[65]. Moreover, this study is based on self-reported data, which can be particularly problematic regarding height, weight, and food intake [66, 67]. No instructions were given for how height and weight should be measured, which adds another layer of uncertainty to the data. One possibility for handling self-reported BMI data is to use standardised factors for corrections [66, 68]. Such factors are of great value for improving precision in prevalence numbers based on self-reports. However, prevalence numbers as such were not the aim of this study, but rather associations with eating alone (or not). Food intake was also based on frequencies of food groups, which means that we cannot comment on absolute quantities or fine-grained dietary details (e.g., exactly which fruits and vegetables or which types of fish or shellfish). Further research is, therefore, warranted in order to test differentiated constructs of eating alone in relation to the food-related outcomes that are of particular clinical or public health interest.
Conclusion
The present study consideres the differentiation between objective and subjective constructs when studying eating alone and its relationship with food-related outcomes among 70-听to听75-year-old people in Sweden. In this sample of independent healthy individuals in an early phase of retirement, over a quarter of the respondents were eating alone, but few were bothered by this. There were no associations between the frequency of eating alone and level of healthiness of diet (food index scores), BMI, or intake frequency of fruits and berries, or fish and shellfish. However, people eating alone reported eating vegetables and snacks, sugary foods, and sweet drinks less often, consuming ready-made meals more often, and eating fewer main meals per day than those eating together. Eating alone, therefore, first and foremost seemed to be a predictor of outcomes related to the organisation of everyday eating routines, and indicating a simplification of cooking and eating. The subjective experience of eating alone had largely no impact on food-related outcomes. However, individuals bothered by eating alone are likely to be statistically underrepresented in our sample, which hinders our possibilities to draw conclusions in this regard. Despite the uncertainties raised, our findings add to the previous body of research on commensality, eating alone, and health among the older population, and provide insights into the development of future health policies and research.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
J枚nsson H, Michaud M, Neuman N. What is commensality? A critical discussion of an expanding research field. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(12).
Ministry of Health of Brazil. Dietary guidelines for the Brazilian population. Bras铆lia, Brazil: Secretariat of Health Care, Primary Health Care Department; 2015.
Health Canada. Healthy eating for seniors [Internet]. Canada. 2024 [cited 2024-05-21]. .
Takeda W, Melby MK, Ishikawa Y. Who eats with family and how often? Household members and work styles influence frequency of family meals in urban Japan. Appetite. 2018;125:160鈥71.
Dunbar RIM. Breaking Bread: the functions of social eating. Adapt Hum Behav Physiol. 2017;3(3):198鈥211.
Fischler C. Commensality, society and culture. Soc Sci Inf. 2011;50(3鈥4):528鈥48.
Vesnaver E, Keller HH. Social influences and eating behavior in later life: a review. J Nutr Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;30(1):2鈥23.
de Castro JM. Age-related changes in the social, psychological, and temporal influences on food intake in free-living, healthy, adult humans. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci. 2002;57(6):M368鈥77.
Keller HH, Goy R, Kane SL. Validity and reliability of SCREEN II (seniors in the community: risk evaluation for eating and nutrition, Version II). Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005;59(10):1149鈥57.
Bj枚rnwall A, Sydner YM, Koochek A, Neuman N. Eating alone or together among community-living older people鈥攁 scoping review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(7):3495.
Moon JH, Huh JS, Won CW, Kim HJ. Living and eating alone on depressive symptoms by physical frailty status: a cross-sectional study based on the Korean Frailty and aging cohort study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2022;98:104570鈥104570.
Li CL, Tung HJ, Yeh MC. Combined effect of eating alone and a poor nutritional status on cognitive decline among older adults in Taiwan. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2018;27(3):686鈥94.
Ishikawa M, Takemi Y, Yokoyama T, Kusama K, Fukuda Y, Nakaya T, et al. Eating together鈥 is associated with food behaviors and demographic factors of older Japanese people who live alone. J Nutr Health Aging. 2017;21(6):662鈥72.
Kimura Y, Wada T, Okumiya K, Ishimoto Y, Fukutomi E, Kasahara Y, et al. Eating alone among community-dwelling Japanese elderly: association with depression and food diversity. J Nutr Health Aging. 2012;16(8):728鈥31.
Tani Y, Kondo N, Takagi D, Saito M, Hikichi H, Ojima T, et al. Combined effects of eating alone and living alone on unhealthy dietary behaviors, obesity and underweight in older Japanese adults: results of the JAGES. Appetite. 2015;95:1鈥8.
Locher JL, Robinson CO, Roth DL, Ritchie CS, Burgio KL. The effect of the presence of others on caloric intake in homebound older adults J Gerontol A Biol Sci. Med Sci. 2005;60(11):1475鈥8.
Sidenvall B, Nydahl M, Fjellstr枚m C. The meal as a gift鈥攖he meaning of cooking among retired women. J Appl Gerontol. 2000;19(4):405鈥23.
Vesnaver E, Keller HH, Sutherland O, Maitland SB, Locher JL. Alone at the table: food behavior and the loss of commensality in widowhood. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2016;71(6):1059鈥69.
Mattsson Sydner Y, Fjellstr枚m C, Lumbers M, Sidenvall B, Raats M. Food habits and foodwork. Food Cult Soc. 2007;10(3):367鈥87.
Bj枚rnwall A, Mattsson Sydner Y, Koochek A, Neuman N. Perceptions and experiences of eating alone among community-living retired swedes: loss, routine and independence. Appetite. 2023;106570鈥106570.
Andersen SS. 鈥業鈥檓 old, but I鈥檓 not old-fashioned鈥: mealtimes and cooking practices among Danish widows and widowers. Ageing Soc. 2020;1鈥18.
Neuman N, Gottz茅n L, Fjellstr枚m C. Narratives of progress: cooking and gender equality among Swedish men. J Gend Stud. 2017;26(2):151鈥63.
de Jong-Gierveld J. A review of loneliness: Concept and definitions, determinants and consequences. Rev Clin Gerontol. 1998;8(1):73鈥80.
Coyle CE, Dugan E. Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. J Aging Health. 2012;24(8):1346鈥63.
Lennartsson C, Rehnberg J, Dahlberg L. The association between loneliness, social isolation and all-cause mortality in a nationally representative sample of older women and men. Aging Ment Health. 2022;26(9):1821鈥8.
Daniel LS, Mengyun L, Robert E, Klaus G, van Joseph B, Adrian B, et al. The prevalence of loneliness across 113 countries: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2022;376:e067068.
Fokkema T, de Jong-Gierveld J, Dykstra PA. Cross-national differences in older adult loneliness. J Psychol. 2012;146(1鈥2):201鈥28.
Rapolien臈 G, Aartsen M. Lonely societies: low trust societies? Further explanations for national variations in loneliness among older europeans. Eur J Ageing; 2021.
Statistics Sweden. Hush氓ll i Sverige [Internet]. Sweden. 2022 [cited 2024-05-21]. .
Holm L, Lauridsen D, Lund TB, Gronow J, Niva M, M盲kel盲 J. Changes in the social context and conduct of eating in four nordic countries between 1997 and 2012. Appetite. 2016;103:358鈥68.
Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)鈥擜 metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inf. 2009;42(2):377鈥81.
Public Health Agency of Sweden. N盲r h盲nde vad under pandemin? [Internet]. Sweden, 2023 [cited 2024-05-21]. .
Strandell H, Wolf P, editors. Ageing Europe - looking at the lives of older people in the EU. Luxembourg: Eurostat; 2020.
Skoog I. Rapport 21.70 盲r det nya 50 [Internet]. Delegationen f枚r senior arbetskraft; 2018. S2018:10 [cited 2024-05-21]. Avaliable from: .
Charan J, Biswas T. How to calculate sample size for different study designs in medical research? Indian J Psychol Med. 2013;35(2):121鈥6.
Public Health Agency of Sweden. Fakta om nationella folkh盲lsoenk盲ten H盲lsa p氓 lika villkor? [Internet]. Public Health Agency of Sweden. 2024. [cited 2024-05-21]. .
Lennartsson C, Agahi N, Hols-Sal茅n L, Kelfve S, K氓reholt I, Lundberg O, et al. Data resource profile: the Swedish panel study of living conditions of the oldest old (SWEOLD). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(3):731鈥8.
Public Health Agency of Sweden. Syfte och bakgrund till fr氓gorna i nationella folkh盲lsoenk盲ten. Folkh盲lsomyndigheten. 2018. 18083.
Wolk A. COSM - a cohort of 50,000 Swedish men [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2024-05-21]. Avaliable from:
Willis B. Cognitive interviewing: a tool for improving questionnaire design. Sage; 2004.
Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare. Utdrag ur indikatorbilagan f枚r Nationella Riktlinjer f枚r sjukdomsf枚rebyggande metoder 2011. Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare; 2020.
Sepp H, Ekelund U, Becker W. Enk盲tfr氓gor Om Kost och fysisk aktivitet bland vuxna. Swedish National Food Agency; 2004. Rapport 21.
Huseinovic E, Winkvist A, Freisling H, Slimani N, Boeing H, Buckland G, et al. Timing of eating across ten european countries - results from the European prospective investigation into cancer and nutrition (EPIC) calibration study. Public Health Nutr. 2019;22(2):324鈥35.
Yates L, Warde A. Eating together and eating alone: meal arrangements in British households. Br J Sociol. 2017;68(1):97鈥118.
R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2023.
Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, McGowan LD, Fran莽ois R, et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. J Open Source Softw. 2019;4(43):1686.
Wickham H. ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer; 2016.
Long JA. jtools: Analysis and presentation of social scientific data. R package version 2.2.0; 2022.
Fox J, Weisberg S, An R companion to applied regression. Third edition. Sage, Thousand Oaks CA.; 2019.
Christensen RHB. Ordinal - Regression models for Ordinal Data. R package version 2022.11-16.; 2022.
Mangiafico SS, rcompanion. Functions to support extension education program evaluationversion 2.4.34. Rutgers cooperative extension. New Brunswick, New Jersey; 2023.
Mackenbach JP. Health inequalities: persistence and change in European welfare states [Internet]. Oxford University Press; 2019 [cited 2024 May 17]. .
Beller J, Wagner A. Loneliness and health: the moderating effect of cross-cultural individualism/collectivism. J Aging Health. 2020;32(10):1516鈥27.
World Values Survey Association. Findings & insights [Internet]. 2020. Avaliable from: .
J枚nsson H. One hundred years of solitude and commensality: a study of cooking and meals in Swedish households. In: Bartsch S, Lysaght P, editors. Places of food production: origin, identity, imagination. Peter Lang Publishing Group; 2017, pp. 259鈥72.
Neuman N, Gottz茅n L, Fjellstr枚m C. Masculinity and the sociality of cooking in men鈥檚 everyday lives. Sociol Rev. 2017;65(4):816鈥31.
Neuman N, J枚rnvi A, Ek A, Nordin K, Eli K, Nowicka P. Children鈥檚 experiences of meals after obesity treatment: a qualitative follow-up four years after a randomized controlled trial. 樱花视频 Pediatr. 2022;22(1):342.
Persson Osowski C, Mattsson Sydner Y. The family meal as an ideal: children鈥檚 perceptions of foodwork and commensality in everyday life and feasts. Int J Consum Stud. 2019;43(2):178鈥86.
Andersen SS, Br眉nner RN. New roads to commensality in widowhood. Appetite. 2020;155:104827鈥104827.
Lund TB, Gronow J. Destructuration or continuity? The daily rhythm of eating in Denmark, Finland, Norway and Sweden in 1997 and 2012. Appetite. 2014;82:143鈥53.
Ruddock HK, Brunstrom JM, Vartanian LR, Higgs S. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the social facilitation of eating. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019;110(4):842鈥61.
Kusama T, Kiuchi S, Tani Y, Aida J, Kondo K, Osaka K. The lack of opportunity to eat together is associated with an increased risk of weight loss among independent older adults: a prospective cohort study based on the JAGES. Age Ageing. 2022;51(3).
Nicklett EJ, Johnson KE, Troy LM, Vartak M, Reiter A. Food access, diet quality, and nutritional status of older adults during COVID-19: a scoping review. Front Public Health. 2021;9.
Scafoglieri A, Provyn S, Bautmans I, Van Roy P, Clarys JP. Direct relationship of body mass index and waist circumference with body tissue distribution in elderly persons. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15(10):924鈥31.
Winter JE, MacInnis RJ, Wattanapenpaiboon N, Nowson CA. BMI and all-cause mortality in older adults: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(4):875鈥90.
Connor Gorber S, Shields M, Tremblay MS, McDowell I. The feasibility of establishing correction factors to adjust self-reported estimates of obesity. Health Rep. 2008;19(3):71鈥82.
Subar AF, Freedman LS, Tooze JA, Kirkpatrick SI, Boushey C, Neuhouser ML, et al. Addressing current criticism regarding the value of self-report dietary data. J Nutr. 2015;145(12):2639鈥45.
Koochek A, Johansson SE, Kocturk TO, Sundquist J, Sundquist K. Physical activity and body mass index in elderly iranians in Sweden: a population-based study. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008;62(11):1326鈥32.
Acknowledgements
We are deeply grateful to all respondents for taking the time and effort to answer the survey. Thanks to Professor Agneta Yngve for contributions at the beginning of the research project, to Associate Professor Afsaneh Koochek for contributions at the beginning of this particular study, and to colleagues at the Department of Food Studies, Nutrition and Dietetics for constructive feedback, and valuable suggestions to improve this paper.
Funding
This research was supported by Stiftelsen Kronprinsessan Margarets Minne.
Open access funding provided by Uppsala University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
AB and NN conceptualized the study. AB performed data collection, and AB and PEC performed data analyses. The first draft of the manuscript was written by AB. AB, PEC, YMS, and NN read, revised and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Approval for this study was sought from and approved by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (Dnr 2021鈥夆垝鈥01988). Informed consent was obtained from all respondents.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher鈥檚 Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article鈥檚 Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article鈥檚 Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver () applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Bj枚rnwall, A., Colombo, P.E., Sydner, Y.M. et al. The impact of eating alone on food intake and everyday eating routines: A cross-sectional study of community-living 70-听to听75-year-olds in Sweden. 樱花视频 24, 2214 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-19560-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-19560-0